Advertisement

Suppression from the Perspective of Distributed Models of Conceptual Representation

  • Omid Khatin-Zadeh
  • Hooshang Khoshsima
  • Nahid Yarahmadzehi
Ideas and Opinion
  • 3 Downloads

Abstract

Looking at metaphor comprehension from the perspective of distributed models of conceptual representation, this article describes the process of suppression through which metaphorically irrelevant features of metaphor’s vehicle are suppressed. Distributed models of conceptual representation hold that meaning of every concept is represented by a set of feature nodes in a connectionist network, and the coactivation of these nodes leads to the understanding of that concept. Based on these models, it is suggested that degree of distinctiveness of features play an important role in the suppression of metaphorically irrelevant features during metaphor comprehension. When the metaphor X is a Y is processed, a salient feature of Y creates a metaphorical class to which both topic (X) and vehicle (Y) belong. The rest of features, which are metaphorically irrelevant, are suppressed. Those irrelevant features which have a high degree of correlational strength are suppressed collectively. Finally, the key role of metaphor’s topic in the suppression of metaphorically irrelevant features is discussed. It is suggested that the set of suppressed features is dependent on the topic. If the defining feature of the metaphorical class of the vehicle (Y) matches the topic (X), the rest of features will be suppressed. In this situation, both topic and vehicle of the metaphor are included in a common metaphorical class. If the defining feature of metaphorical class of the vehicle (Y) does not match the topic (X), X is understood in its literal sense, and the sentence X is a Y will not have a logical metaphorical interpretation.

Keywords

Metaphor comprehension Distributed models Suppression Connectionist network Topic Vehicle 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Banaruee, H., Khoshsima, H., Khatin-Zadeh, O., & Askari, A. (2017). Suppression of semantic features in metaphor comprehension. Cogent Psychology, 4(1), 1409323.  https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2017.1409323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Caramazza, A., Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B. C., & Romani, C. (1990). The multiple semantics hypothesis: multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cree, G. S., McNorgan, C., & McRae, K. (2006). Distinctive features hold a privileged status in the computation of word meaning: implications for theories of semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(4), 643–658.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Cree, G. S., & McRae, K. (2003). Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese, and cello (and many other such concrete nouns). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 163–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Devlin, J. T., Gonnerman, L. M., Andersen, E. S., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Category-specific semantic deficits in focal and widespread brain damage: a computational account. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 77–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gernsbacher, M. A. (1989). Mechanisms that improve referential access. Cognition, 32, 99–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gernsbacher, M, A., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1995). Reading skill and suppression revisited. Psychological Science, 6, 165–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1999). The role of suppression in figurative language comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1619–1630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gernsbacher, M, A., Shroyer, S. (1989). The cataphoric use of the indefinite this in spoken narratives. Memory and Cognition, 17, 536–540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: salience, context and figurative language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195136166.001.0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Giora, R. (2006). Anything negatives can do affirmatives can do just as well, except for some metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 981–1014.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2005.12.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Giora, R. (2007). “A good Arab is not a dead Arab—A racist incitement”: on the accessibility of negated concepts. In I. Kecskes & L. R. Horn (Eds.), Explorations in pragmatics: linguistic, cognitive and intercultural aspects (pp. 129–162). Berlin, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  13. Giora, R., Balaban, N., Fein, O., & Alkabets, I. (2005a). Negation as positivity in disguise. In H. L. Colston & A. Katz (Eds.), Figurative language comprehension: social and cultural influences (pp. 233–258). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  14. Giora, R., Fein, O., Ganzi, J., Levi, N. A., & Sabah, H. (2005b). Negation as mitigation: the case of negative irony. Discourse Processes, 39, 81–100.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3901_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Giora, R., Fein, O., Metuki, N., & Stern, P. (2010). Negation as a metaphor-inducing operator. In L. Horn (Ed.), The expression of negation (pp. 225–256). Berlin, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  16. Giora, R., Livnat, E., Fein, O., Barnea, A., Zeiman, R., & Berger, I. (2013). Negation generates nonliteral interpretations by default. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 89–115.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2013.768510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Glucksberg, S., Newsome, M. R., & Goldvarg, Y. (2001). Inhibition of the literal: filtering metaphor-irrelevant information during metaphor comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 277–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Keil, F. (1986). The acquisition of natural kinds and artifact terms. In Demoupoulous, W., Marras, A., eds. Language Learning and Concept Acquisition: Foundational Issues. Ablex; Norwood, NJ, 133–53.Google Scholar
  19. Keysar, B. (1994). Discourse context effects: metaphorical and literal interpretations. Discourse Processes, 18, 247–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Khatin-Zadeh, O., & Vahdat, S. (2015). Abstract and concrete representation in structure-mapping and class-inclusion. Cognitive Linguistic Studies, 2(2), 349–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Masson, M. (1995). A distributed memory model of semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McRae, K., Cree, G. S. (2002). Factors underlying category-specific semantic deficits. In Forde, EME., Humphreys, G. Category-specificity in mind and brain. Psychology Press, East Sussex, 211–50.Google Scholar
  23. McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 37, 547–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Westmacott, R., & Sa, V. R. (1999). Further evidence for feature correlations in semantic memory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(4), 360–373.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087323.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. McRae, K., de Sa, V. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(2), 99–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Moss, H. E., Tyler, L. K., & Delvin, J. (2002). The emergence of category specific defecits in a distributed semantic system. In E. Forde & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 115–148). Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  27. Moss, H. E., Tyler, L. K., & Taylor, K. I. (2007). Conceptual structure. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 217–234). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Pynte, J., Besson, M., Robichon, F. H., & Poli, J. (1996). The time-course of metaphor comprehension: an event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 55(3), 293–316.  https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1996.0107.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Taylor, K. I., Devereux, B. J., & Tyler, L. K. (2011). Conceptual structure: towards an integrated neurocognitive account. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(9), 1368–1401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Tyler, L. K., Durrant-Peatfield, M. R., Levy, J. P., Voice, J. K., & Moss, H. E. (1996). Distinctiveness and correlations in the structure of categories: behavioral data and a connectionist model. Brain and Language, 55, 89–91.Google Scholar
  32. Tyler, L. K., & Moss, H. E. (2001). Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 244–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Tyler, L. K., Moss, H. E., Durrant-Peatfield, M. R., & Levy, J. P. (2000). Conceptual structure and the structure of concepts: a distributed account of category-specific deficits. Brain and Language, 75, 195–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D., Lewis, W., & Garrett, M. (2004). Representing the meanings of object and action words: the featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology. Google Scholar
  35. Vinson, D. P., Vigliocco, G., Cappa, S., & Siri, S. (2003). The breakdown of semantic knowledge: insights from a statistical model of meaning representation. Brain and Language, 86, 347–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Neuroscientia 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Chabahar Maritime UniversityChabaharIran

Personalised recommendations