Advertisement

The Effect of Complexity on Training for Exploration of Non-intuitive Rules in Theory of Mind

  • Nirit Yuviler-GavishEmail author
  • Doron Faran
  • Mark Berman
Original Research
  • 10 Downloads

Abstract

The current research focused on training to enhance exploration in Theory of Mind (ToM), using a training program based on the game SET®. (© 1988, 1991 Cannei, LLC. All rights reserved. SET® and all associated logos and taglines are registered trademarks of Cannei, LLC. Used with permission from Set Enterprises, Inc.) Two experimental groups were tasked with predicting the selections of a virtual player given a set of unknown rules governing the assignment of values to SETs, where one aspect of the rules (the fact that some values were negative) was designed to be particularly unintuitive. In the Simple Rule group, there were only four sets of values and their assignment followed a pattern, whereas in the Complex Rule group, there were many sets of values and their assignment was arbitrary, requiring greater exploration to determine them. The Simple Rule group was better at predicting more-intuitive selections of the virtual player, while the Complex Rule group was both better and faster at predicting less-intuitive selections. Hence, exposing trainees to complex rules governing others’ decisions might be used to change people’s tendency toward under-exploration in ToM.

Keywords

Decision-making Learning Games Cognitive structure Knowledge 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by the ORT Braude College, Israel.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. The research involved human participants and was approved by the ORT Braude College’s ethical committee. Participants have signed on informed consent.

References

  1. Ashcroft, A., Jervis, N., & Roberts, C. (1999). A theory of mind (TOM) and people with learning disabilities: the effects of a training package. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 12, 58–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical explanations of children’s understanding of the mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bazerman, M., & Neal, M. (1992). Negotiating rationally. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  4. Benson, G., Abbeduto, L., Short, K., Bibler Nuccio, J., & Mass, F. (1993). Development of a Theory of Mind in individuals with mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 98, 427–433.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Brooks, L. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Gao, X., & Wilson, H. R. (2014). Implicit learning of geometric eigenfaces. Vision Research, 99, 12–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gopher, D. (2007). Emphasis change as a training protocol for high-demands tasks. In A. Kramer, D. Wiegman, & A. Kirlik (Eds.), Applied attention: from theory to practice. Oxford University Press: New York, USA.Google Scholar
  8. Gopher, D., Weil, M., & Siegel, D. (1989). Practice under changing priorities: an approach to training of complex skills. Acta Psychologica, 71, 147–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gordon, P. C., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Implicit learning and generalization of the “mere exposure” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 492–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. Developmental Science, 6, 346–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Harbers, M., Van den Bosch, K., & Meyer, J. (2009). Enhancing training by using agents with a theory of mind. Proceedings of EduMas, 23–30.Google Scholar
  12. Hodges, F. B. (1993). Training for uncertainty. Fort Leavenworth, KS: School of Advanced Military Studies.Google Scholar
  13. Hoogendoorn, M., & Merk, R. J. (2012). Action selection using theory of mind: a case study in the domain of fighter pilot training. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth International Conference on Industrial, Engineering & Other Applications of Applied Intelligent Systems, IEA-AIE 2012, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 7345. Springer Verlag (pp. 521–533).
  14. Jacob, M., & Hochstein, S. (2008). SET recognition as a window to perceptual and cognitive processes. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(7), 1165–1184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: implications for theory of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 144–162). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: the role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11, 32–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Light Jr., D. (1979). Uncertainty and control in professional training. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 20, 310–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McAndrews, M. P., & Moscovitch, M. (1985). Rule-based and exemplar-based classification in artificial grammar learning. Memory and Cognition, 13, 469–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. McDougle, S. D., Bond, K. M., & Taylor, J. A. (2015). Explicit and implicit processes constitute the fast and slow processes of sensorimotor learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(26), 9568–9579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Melot, A. M., & Angeard, N. (2003). Theory of mind: is training contagious? Developmental Science, 6, 178–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Nyamsuren, E., & Taatgen, N. A. (2013a). SET as an instance of a real-world visual-cognitive task. Cognitive Science, 37(1), 146–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nyamsuren, E., & Taatgen, N. A. (2013b). The effect of visual representation style in problem-solving: a perspective from cognitive processes. PLoS One, 8(11), e80550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nyamsuren, E., & Taatgen, N. A. (2013c). The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3181–3186).Google Scholar
  24. Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: a new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 415–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  26. Perner, J. (1999). Theory of mind. In M. Bennett (Ed.), Developmental psychology (pp. 205–230). London: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
  27. Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. (1990). Autistic children’s knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 587–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Prosser, B. D. (1996). The need for training for uncertainty. Fort Leavenworth, KS: U.S. Army Command and General Staff College, School of Advanced Military Studies.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6(6), 855–863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: the role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 88–94.Google Scholar
  31. Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(3), 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Reber, A. S., Allen, A., & Regan, S. (1985). Synthetic learning and judgment, still unconscious and still abstract: comment on Dulany, Carlson, and Dewey. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 17–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Reber, A. S., & Millward, R. B. (1965). Probability learning and memory for event sequences. Psychonomic Science, 3, 431–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Reed, T., & Peterson, C. (1990). A comparative study of autistic participants’ performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 555–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Seagull, F. J., & Gopher, D. (1997). Training head movement in visual scanning: an embedded approach to the development of piloting skills with helmet-mounted displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 163–180.Google Scholar
  36. Slaughter, V., & Gopnik, A. (1996). Conceptual coherence in the child’s theory of mind: training children to understand belief. Child Development, 67, 2967–2988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sodian, B., & Frith, U. (1992). Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 591–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sodian, B., & Kristen, S. (2010). Theory of mind. In B. M. Glatzeder, V. Goel, & A. v. Müller (Eds.), Towards a theory of thinking (pp. 189–201). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  39. Taatgen, N. A., van Oploo, M., Braaksma, J., & Niemantsverdriet, J. (2003). How to construct a believable opponent using cognitive modeling in the game of SET. In Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (pp. 201-206).Google Scholar
  40. Thompson, L. (1991). Information exchange in negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 161–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Vandenbossche, J., Coomans, D., Homblé, K., & Deroost, N. (2014). The effect of cognitive aging on implicit sequence learning and dual tasking. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(154), 1–7.Google Scholar
  42. Wellman, H. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  43. Wellman, H., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2000). Developing understandings of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (Second ed., pp. 21–49). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Yechiam, E., Erev, I., & Gopher, D. (2001). On value and limitation of emphasis change and other exploration enhancing training methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 277–285.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Yechiam, E., Erev, I., Yehene, V., & Gopher, D. (2004). Melioration and the transition from touch typing training to everyday use. Human Factors, 45, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Industrial Engineering and ManagementORT Braude CollegeKarmielIsrael
  2. 2.Department of MathematicsORT Braude CollegeKarmielIsrael

Personalised recommendations