Advertisement

Sleep and Biological Rhythms

, Volume 14, Issue 3, pp 279–285 | Cite as

Expectation of a loud alarm is not associated with changes in on-call sleep in the laboratory

  • Sarah M. JayEmail author
  • Brad Aisbett
  • Sally A. Ferguson
Original Article

Abstract

Anecdotally, people report disturbed sleep when ‘on-call’ and field data suggest that being on-call, even if ‘undisturbed’, may result in sleep disturbance. We investigated changes to sleep when expecting a loud, on-call alarm as compared to sleep when not expecting an alarm. Healthy males (n = 16) aged 24.6 ± 4.0 years took part in a simulated on-call scenario involving two conditions; Control and on-call. Prior to the Control sleep, participants were told that they would not be woken during the night, prior to the on-call sleep, participants were told to expect a loud alarm during the night, following which they were to complete 2 h of testing. Sleep was measured using a standard 5-channel polysomnograhic (PSG) montage. Sleep diaries were used to compare subjective variables; pre- and post-sleep sleepiness and sleep quality. There was no significant difference between the two nights for any of the PSG variables, except for REM where there was a non-significant trend (p = .051) with 8 min more REM on the on-call night. Participants were significantly sleepier following the on-call night, likely due to the earlier wake time (p < .01). These results question whether simply being on-call is disruptive to sleep or whether disruption is connected to other factors such as likelihood of being called, worry about missing the call and/or the events that follow.

Keywords

Alarm On-call Simulation Perceived sleep quality Sleep Stress 

Notes

Acknowledgements/disclosure

There are no conflicts of interests to declare. We would like to thank and for the staff and students at CQUniversity and Deakin University for their time and efforts during data collection. This Research was supported by the CQUniversity Research and Development Incentives Program and in part, by a Deakin University, Faculty of Health Research Development Grant.

References

  1. 1.
    Fifth Europrean Working Conditions Survey. Overview report. European foundation for the improvement of living and working conditions. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union; 2012. p. 151.Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Australian Bureau of Statistics. Forms of Employment. ABS, Canberra; 2012.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Rose M, Manser T, Ware JC. Effects of call on sleep and mood in internal medicine residents. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6:75–88.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Jay SM, Thomas MJW, Weissenfeld AP, Dawson D, Ferguson SA. Doctor’s sleep disturbance when on-call. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2008;6(sppl1):A32.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Arora VM, Georgitis E, Siddique J, Vekhter B, Woodruff JN, Humphrey HJ, et al. Association of workload of on-call medical interns with on-call sleep duration, shift duration, and participation in educational activities. JAMA. 2008;300(10):1146–53.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Lockley SW, Cronin JW, Evans EE, Cade BE, Lee CJ, Landrigan CP, et al. Effect of reducing interns’ weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures. New Engl J Med. 2004;351(18):1829–37.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The Cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioural functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26(2):117–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, Thomas ML, Sing HC, Redmond DP, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12(1):1–12.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, Van Cauter E. The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(3):163–78.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12(3):197–210.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Vandekerckhove M, Cluydts R. The emotional brain and sleep: an intimate relationship. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(4):219–26.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Torsvall L, Akerstedt T. Disturbed sleep while being on-call: an EEG study of ships’ engineers. Sleep. 1988;11(1):35–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Akerstedt T. Psychosocial stress and impaired sleep. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2006;32(6):493–501.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Akerstedt T, Kecklund G, Axelsson J. Impaired sleep after bedtime stress and worries. Biol Psychol. 2007;76(3):170–3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Torsvall L, Castenfors K, Akerstedt T, Froberg J. Sleep at Sea. A diary study of the effects of unattended machinery space watch duty. Ergonomics. 1987;30:1335–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Wuyts J, De Valck E, Vandekerckhove M, Pattyn N, Exadaktylos V, Haex B, et al. Effects of pre-sleep simulated on-call instructions on subsequent sleep. Biol Psychol. 2012;91(3):383–8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Richardson GS, James K, Wyatt JK, Sullivan JP, Orav EJ, Ward AE, Wolf MA, et al. Objective assessment of sleep and alertness i.n medical house staff and the impact of protected time for sleep. Sleep. 1996;19(9):718–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Barnes P. The experience of traumatic stress among urban firefighters. Aust J Emerg Manage. 2000;14(4):59–60.Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Kuorinka I, Korhonen O. Firefighters’ reaction to alarm, an ECG and heart rate study. J Occup Med Off Publ Ind Med Assoc. 1981;23(11):762–6.Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Cuddy JS, Gaskill SE, Sharkey BJ, Harger SG, Ruby BC. Supplemental feedings increase self-selected work output during wildfire suppression. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(6):1004–12.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Cuddy JS, Sol JA, Hailes WS, Ruby BC. Work patterns dictate energy demands and thermal strain during wildland firefighting. Wilderness Environ Med. 2015.Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Buysse DJ, Reynolds CFI, Monk TH, Bermen SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1988;28:193–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A, Quan S. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. 1st ed. Westchester: Amercian Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007.Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement WC. Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology. 1973;10(4):431–6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Suchecki D, Tiba PA, Machado RB. REM sleep rebound as an adaptive response to stressful situations. Front Neurol. 2012;3:41.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–96.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biol Psychol. 2005;69(1):113–32.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Kajantie E, Phillips DI. The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(2):151–78.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japanese Society of Sleep Research 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sarah M. Jay
    • 1
    Email author
  • Brad Aisbett
    • 2
  • Sally A. Ferguson
    • 1
  1. 1.Appleton InstituteCQUniversityAdelaideAustralia
  2. 2.School of Exercise and Nutrition SciencesDeakin UniversityBurwoodAustralia

Personalised recommendations