Evolutionary Psychological Science

, Volume 3, Issue 4, pp 392–403 | Cite as

Why is Intelligence Negatively Associated with Religiousness?

Theoretical Article

Abstract

We present three models which attempt to explain the robust negative association between religion and intelligence: the Irrationality of Religion Model, the Cultural Mediation Hypothesis, and the Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis. We highlight problems with each of them and propose that the negative religion-IQ nexus can be understood through substantially revising the Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis. We argue that religion should be regarded as an evolved domain or instinct. Intelligence, by contrast, involves rising above our instincts. It follows that an inclination toward the non-instinctive will thus be an aspect of intelligence because it will help us to solve problems. Thus, intelligence will involve being attracted to evolutionary mismatch, to that which we would not be instinctively evolved to be attracted to. It is this, we argue, that is behind the negative religion-intelligence nexus. We respond to potential criticisms of our model and we examine how this model can be further tested.

Keywords

Religion Cultural Mediation Hypothesis Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis Evolutionary mismatch Intelligence 

References

  1. BaHammam, A. (2011). Sleep from an Islamic perspective. Annals of Thoracic Medicine, 6, 187–192.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett, J. (2004). Why would anyone believe in god? Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bender, I. (1968). A longitudinal study of church attenders and non-attenders. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 7, 230–237. doi:10.2307/1384630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blume, M. (2009). The reproductive benefits of religious affiliation. In E. Voland & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  5. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: the human instincts that fashion gods, spirits and ancestors. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  6. Brierley, P. (2010). UK Christianity: 2005–2015. London: Christian Research Association.Google Scholar
  7. Browne, T. (1672). Pseudoxia epidemica: or enquiries into the very many received tenets and commonly presumed truths. London: Printed for Edward Dod and are to be Sould (sic.) by Andrew Crook.Google Scholar
  8. Cofnas, N. (2012). Reptiles with a conscience: the co-evolution of religious and moral doctrine. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  9. Cofnas, N. (2016). A teleofunctional account of everyday evolutionary mismatch. Biology and Philosophy, 31, 507–525.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Cornelis, I., Van Heil, A., Roets, A., & Kassowska, M. (2008). Age differences in conservatism: evidence on the mediating effects of personality and cognitive style. Journal of Personality, 77, 51–88.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. DeYoung, C., Peterson, J., & Higgins, D. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73, 825–858.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dunkel, C., Reeve, C., Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Van der Linden, D. (2015). A comparative study of the general factor of personality in Jewish and non-Jewish populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 78, 63–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Durrant, R., & Ellis, B. (2003). Evolutionary psychology. In M. Gallagher & R. Nelson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: volume I: biological psychology. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Dutton, E. (2013a). The cultural mediation hypotheses: a critical examination. Intelligence, 41, 321–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dutton, E. (2013b). The Savanna-IQ Interaction hypothesis: a critical examination of the comprehensive case presented in Kanazawa’s The Intelligence Paradox. Intelligence, 41, 607–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dutton, E. (2014). Religion and intelligence: an evolutionary analysis. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  17. Dutton, E., & Charlton, B. (2015). The genius famine: why we need geniuses, why they are dying out and why we must rescue them. Buckingham: Buckingham University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Dutton, E., & Lynn, R. (2014). Intelligence and religious and political differences among the US academic elite. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion, 10, 1.Google Scholar
  19. Dutton, E., & Van der Linden, D. (2015). Who are the “clever sillies”? The intelligence, personality, and motives of clever silly originators and those who follow them. Intelligence, 49, 57–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dutton, E., Van der Linden, D., & Lynn, R. (2016a). The negative Flynn effect: a systematic literature review. Intelligence, 59, 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dutton, E., Madison, G., & Lynn, R. (2016b). Demographic, economic, and genetic factors related to national differences in ethnocentric attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 137–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Eisenberg, N. (2012). Temperamental effortful control (self-regulation). Encyclopedia on early childhood development, http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/temperament/according-experts/temperamental-effortful-control-self-regulation.
  23. Ellis, L., Hoskin, A., Dutton, E., & Nyborg, H. (2017). The future of secularism: a biologically informed theory supplemented with cross-cultural evidence. Evolutionary Psychological Science. doi:10.1007/s40806-017-0090-z.Google Scholar
  24. Farias, M. (2013). The psychology of atheism. In S. Bullivant & M. Ruse (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of atheism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Feather, N. T. (1967). Evaluation of religious and neutral arguments in religious and atheist student groups. Australian Journal of Psychology, 19, 3–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Figueredo, A., Vasquez, G., Brumbach, B., et al. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: from genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26, 243–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Flynn, J. (2012). Are we getting smarter? Rising IQ in the twenty-first century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gaskill, M. (2003). Crime and mentalities in early modern England. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Gilkey, C. K. (1924). Religion among American students. Journal of Religion, 1, 4.Google Scholar
  30. Goodey, C. F. (2011). A history of intelligence and “intellectual disability”: the shaping of psychology in early modern Europe. Farnham: Ashgate.Google Scholar
  31. Gordon, B., & Marshall, P. (Eds.). (2000). The place of the dead: death and remembrance in late medieval and early modern Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Hammond, R., & Axelrod, R. (2006). The evolution of ethnocentric behaviour. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hauschild, T. (2011). Power and Magic in Italy. Oxford: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
  34. Howells, T. (1928). A comparative study of those who accept as against those who reject religious authority. University of Iowa Studies in Character, 2, 3.Google Scholar
  35. Hudson, W. (2015). The English deists: studies in early in Enlightenment. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  36. James, W. (1896/1956). The will to believe and other essays in popular philosophy. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
  37. James, W. (1907). Pragmatism: a new name for some old ways of thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jenkins, T. (2009). Faith and the scientific mind/faith in the scientific mind: the implicit religion of science in contemporary Britain. Implicit Religion, 12, 3.Google Scholar
  39. Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
  40. Jordan, J. (2013). Pragmatic arguments and belief in God. Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatic-belief-god/#PraArg.
  41. Kanazawa, S. (2012). The intelligence paradox: why the intelligent choice isn’t always the smart one. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  42. Kay, A. C., Shepherd, S., Blatz, C. W., et al. (2010). For god (or) country: the hydraulic relation between government instability and belief in religious sources of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 725–739.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. King, C. (2008). Faustus and the Promise of the New Science, c. 1580-1730: From the Chapbooks to Harlequin Faustus. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
  44. Koenig, L., McGue, M., Krueger, R., & Bouchard, T. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings. Journal of Personality, 73, 471–478.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Kosa, J., & Schommer, C. (1961). Religious participation, religious knowledge, and scholastic aptitude: an empirical study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1, 88–97. doi:10.2307/1385179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Larsen, R., & Witham, L. (1998). Leading scientists still reject god. Nature, 394, 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lewis, G., Ritchie, S., & Bates, C. (2011). The relationship between intelligence and multiple domains of religious belief: evidence from a large US adult sample. Intelligence, 39, 468–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lynn, R. (2011). Dysgenics: genetic deterioration in modern populations. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  49. Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2012). Intelligence: a unifying construct for the social sciences. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  50. MacDonald, K. (2010). Evolution as a dual processing theory of culture: applications to moral idealism and political philosophy. Politics and Culture, 1: http://politicsandculture.org/2010/04/29/evolution-and-a-dual-processing-theory-of-culture-applications-to-moral-idealism-and-political-philosophy/.
  51. Maroney, E. (2006). Religious syncretism. London: SCM Press.Google Scholar
  52. Marshall, J. (2006). John Locke, toleration and early Enlightenment culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Meisenberg, G. (2007). In God’s image: the natural history of intelligence and ethics. Kibworth: Book Guild Publishing.Google Scholar
  54. Meisenberg, G., Rindermann, H., Patel, H., & Woodley, M. (2012). Is it smart to believe in god? The relationship of religiosity with education and intelligence. Temas em Psicologia, 20, 101–120.Google Scholar
  55. Mill, J. S. (1909). Autobiography. London: P. F. Collier and Son.Google Scholar
  56. Murashko, A. (2013). Air Force Republishes Chaplain’s ‘No Atheists in Foxholes’ Article to Base Website. Christian Post, http://www.christianpost.com/news/air-force-republishes-chaplains-no-atheists-in-foxholes-article-to-base-website-102226/.
  57. Newberg, A., D’Aquili, E., & Rause, V. (2002). Why god won’t go away: brain science and the biology of belief. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
  58. Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious pro-sociality. Science, 322, 58–62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Nyborg, H. (2009). The intelligence-religiosity nexus: a representative study of white adolescent Americans. Intelligence, 37, 81–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Ritchie, S., Gow, A., & Deary, I. (2014). Religiosity is negatively associated with later-life intelligence, but not with age-related cognitive decline. Intelligence, 46, 9–17.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Rushton, J. P. (2000). Race, evolution and behavior: a life history perspective, 3rd special edition. Port Huron: Charles Darwin Institute.Google Scholar
  62. Russell, B. (1952). Is there a God? In: The collected papers of Bertrand Russell, Vol. 11: last philosophical testament, 1943–68. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  63. Saroglou, V. (2002). Religion and the five factors of personality: a meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 15–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Scruton, R. (2000). Modern culture. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
  65. Sedgwick, M. (2009). Against the modern world: traditionalism and the secret intellectual history of the twentieth century. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Snobelen, S. (1999). Isaac Newton, heretic: the strategies of a Nicodemite. British Journal for the History of Science, 32, 381–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Stanovich, K., & West, R. (2008). On the failure of intelligence to predict myside bias and one-sided bias. Thinking & Reasoning, 14, 129–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Steimer, T. (2002). The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 4, 231–249.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Swenson, D. (2008). Religion and family links: Neofunctionalist reflections. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  70. Symonds, J. (1902). Studies of the Greek poets. London: A. & C. Black.Google Scholar
  71. Szobot, C. M., Rohde, L. A., Bukstein, O., Molina, B. S. G., Martins, C., Ruaro, P., & Pechansky, F. (2007). Is attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder associated with illicit substance use disorders in male adolescents? A community-based case-control study. Addiction, 102, 1122–1130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Turchin, P. (2007). War and peace and war: the rise and fall of empires. New York: Plume.Google Scholar
  73. Turner, E. (1980). General cognitive ability and religious attitudes in two school systems. British Journal of Religious Education, 2, 136–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Van der Linden, D., Pekaar, K., Bakker, A., Schermer, J., Vernon, P., Dunkel, C., & Petrides, K. (2016). Overlap between the general factor of personality and emotional intelligence: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. doi:10.1037/bul0000078.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Verhage, F. (1964). Intelligence and religious persuasion. Nederlands tijdschift voor de psychologie en haar grensgebieden, 19, 247–254.Google Scholar
  76. Watson, R. (1994). The rest is silence: death as annihilation in the English renaissance. Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  77. Webster, G., & Duffy, R. (2016). Losing faith in the intelligence-religiosity link: new evidence for a decline effect, spatial dependence, and mediation by education and life quality. Intelligence, 55, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. West, R., Meserve, R., & Stanovich, K. (2012). Cognitive sophistication does not attenuate the bias blind spot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 506–519.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Whitmarsh, T. (2016). Battling the gods: atheism in the ancient world. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
  80. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: the unity of knowledge. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  81. Woodley, M. A. (2010). Are high-IQ individuals deficient in common sense? A critical examination of the ‘clever sillies’ hypothesis. Intelligence, 38, 471–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Dunkel, C. (2015). Beyond the cultural mediation hypothesis: a reply to Dutton (2013). Intelligence, 49, 186–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Fernandes, H. (2015). Do opposing secular trends on backwards and forwards digit span evidence the co-occurrence model? A comment on Gignac (2015). Intelligence, 50, 125–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Woodley, M. A., te Nijenhuis, J., & Murphy, R. (2013). Were the Victorians cleverer than us?: The decline in general intelligence estimated from a meta-analysis of the slowing of simple reaction time. Intelligence, 41, 843–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Young, L. (Ed.). (2016). Rational choice theory and religion: a summary and assessment. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  86. Zuckerman, M., Silverman, J., & Hall, J. A. (2013). The relation between intelligence and religiosity: a meta-analysis and some proposed explanations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 325–354.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ulster Institute for Social ResearchLondonUK
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Education and Child StudiesErasmus University RotterdamRotterdamthe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations