Advertisement

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

, Volume 3, Issue 4, pp 351–364 | Cite as

Facial Trustworthiness is Associated with Heritable Aspects of Face Shape

  • Anthony J. Lee
  • Margaret J. Wright
  • Nicholas G. Martin
  • Matthew C. Keller
  • Brendan P. Zietsch
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Abstract

Facial trustworthiness is thought to underlie social judgements in face perception, though it is unclear whether trustworthiness judgements are based on stable facial attributes. If this were the case, we could expect a genetic component of facial trustworthiness. From facial photographs of a large sample of identical and nonidentical twins and siblings (1320 individuals), we tested for genetic variation in facial trustworthiness and genetic covariation with several stable facial attributes, including facial attractiveness, two measures of masculinity, and facial width-to-height ratio. We found a significant genetic component of facial trustworthiness in men (but not women), and significant genetic correlations with the stable morphological facial traits of attractiveness (positive), perceived masculinity (negative), and facial width-to-height ratio (negative). However, there was no significant genetic or shared environmental correlation between facial trustworthiness and an objective masculinity score based on facial landmark coordinates, despite there being a significant phenotypic correlation. Our results suggest that heritable facial traits influence trustworthiness judgements.

Keywords

Attractiveness Sexual dimorphism Masculinity Facial width-to-height ratio Behavioural genetics Face perception 

Notes

Acknowledgements

AJL has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No 705478. This work was further supported by grants from the Australian Research Council (A79600334, A79801419, DP0212016, FT160100298) and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH085812 and MH63207). Thanks to Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge, Daniel Park, David Smyth, Kerrie McAloney, Natalie Garden, and Reshika Chand; to Courtney Hibbs and Tess Adams for help with data collection; to the professional research assistants at the Center on Antisocial Drug Dependence for their work with the Longitudinal Twin Study; and to the volunteer research assistants who assigned trait ratings. And, thanks to the Queensland Twin (QTwin) Registry and Colorado Twin Registry twins and their families for their continued participation.

Supplementary material

40750_2017_73_MOESM1_ESM.docx (63 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 63 kb)

References

  1. Boker, S., Neale, M. C., Hermine, M., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., et al. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural quation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chang, L., Doll, B. B., van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2010). Seeing is believing: Trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 87–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptions for social exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. DeBruine, L. M. (2005). Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 272(1566), 919–922. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.3003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Watkins, C. D., Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Smith, F. G., & Quist, M. (2011). Opposite-sex siblings decrease attraction, but not prosocial attributions, to self-resembling oppoiste-sex faces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(28), 11710–11714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dotsch, R., & Todorov, A. (2012). Reverse correlating social face perception. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 562–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but...: A meta-analytic review of research on the phsyical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 109–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Ert, E., Fleischer, A., & Magen, N. (2016). Trust and reputation in the sharing economy: The role of personal photos in Airbnb. Tourism Management, 55, 62–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ewing, L., Caulfield, F., Read, A., & Rhodes, G. (2015). Perceived trustworthiness of faces drives trust behaviour in children. Developmental Science, 18(2), 327–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010). Men's facial masculinity predicts changes in their female partners' sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle, whereas men's intelligence does not. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(6), 412–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Haselhuhn, M. P., & Wong, E. M. (2011). Bad to the bone: Facial structure predicts unethical behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences.Google Scholar
  13. Hehman, E., Flake, J., & Freeman, J. B. (2015). Static and dynamic facial cues differentially affect the consistency of social evaluations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 1123–1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kleisner, K., Priplatova, L., Frost, P., & Flegr, J. (2013). Trustworthy-looking face meets brown eyes. PloS One, 8(1), e53285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kramer, R. S. S. (2017). Sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in human skulls and faces: A meta-analytical approach. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(3), 414–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kramer, R. S. S., Jones, A. L., & Ward, R. (2012). A lack of sexual dimorphism in width-to-height ratio in white european faces using 2D photographs, 3D scans, and anthropometry. PloS One, 7(8), e42705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S. R., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7(4), 730–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 390–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lee, A. J., Mitchem, D. G., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., Keller, M. C., & Zietsch, B. P. (2014). Genetic factors increasing male facial masculinity decrease facial attractiveness of female relatives. Psychological Science, 25(2), 476–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lee, A. J., Mitchem, D. G., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., Keller, M. C., & Zietsch, B. P. (2016). Facial averageness and genetic quality: Testing heritability, genetic correlation with attractiveness, and the paternal age effect. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37, 61–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lefevre, C. E., Lewis, G. J., Bates, T. C., Dzhelyova, M., Coetzee, V., Deary, I. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2012). No evidence for sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in four large adult samples. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(6), 623–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Little, A. C., Cohen, D. L., Jones, B. C., & Belsky, J. (2007). Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 967–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Little, A. C., Roberts, S. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2012). The perception of attractiveness and trustworthiness in male faces affects hypothetical voting decisions differently in wartime and peacetime scenarios. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 2018–2032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Loehlin, J. C. (1996). The Cholesky approach: A cautionary note. Behavior Genetics, 26(1), 65–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Maestripieri, D., Henry, A., & Nickels, N. (2017). Explaining financial and prosocial biases in favor of attractive people: Interdisciplinary perspectives from economics, social psychology, and evolutionary psychology. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 40, e19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mattes, K., Spezio, M., Kim, H., Todorov, A., Adolphs, R., & Alvarez, R. M. (2010). Predicting election outcomes from positive and negative trait assessments of candidate images. Political Psychology, 31(1), 41–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mitchem, D. G., Zietsch, B. P., Wright, M. J., Martin, N. G., Hewitt, J. K., & Keller, M. C. (2015). No relationship between intelligence and facial attractiveness in a large, genetically informative sample. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 240–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Pita, M., Arjona, M., Sanchez-Pages, S., & Turiegano, E. (2014). Who is the fairest of them all? The independent effect of attractive features and self-perceived attractiveness on cooperation among women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 118–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Boston: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(32), 11087–11092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2009). Shared perceptual basis of emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from faces. Emotion, 9(1), 128–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Özener, B. (2012). Facial width-to-height ratio in a Turkish population is not sexually dimorphic and is unrelated to aggressive behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(3), 169–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Posthuma, D., Beem, A. L., de Geus, E. J. C., van Baal, G. C. M., von Hjelmborg, J. B., Lachine, I., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Theory and practice in quantitative genetics. Twin Research, 6, 361–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rezlescu, C., Duchaine, B., Olivola, C. Y., & Chater, N. (2012). Unfakeable facial configurations affect strategic choices in trust games with or without information about past behavior. PloS One, 7(3), e34293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rhea, S., Gross, A. A., Haberstick, B. C., & Corley, R. P. (2013). Colorado twin registry - an update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 351–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L. A., Clark, A., Kalick, S. M., Hightower, A., & McKay, R. (2001). Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health? Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(1), 31–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Scott, I. M. L., Pound, N., Stephen, I. D., Clark, A. P., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2010). Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans. PloS One, 5(10), e13585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Shinada, M., & Tamagishi, T. (2014). Physical attractiveness and cooperation in a prisoner's dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 451–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Stirrat, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 21(3), 349–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Surawski, M. K., & Ossoff, E. P. (2006). The effects of physical and vocal attractiveness on impression formation of politicians. Current Psychology, 25(1), 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Takahashi, C., Tamagishi, T., Tanida, S., Kiyonari, T., & Kanazawa, S. (2006). Attractiveness and cooperation in social exchange. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 315–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Todorov, A. (2008). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness: An extension of systems for recognition of emotions signaling approach/avoidance behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 208–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Todorov, A., Pakrashi, M., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2009). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness afater minal time exposure. Social Cognition, 27(6), 813–833.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Verhulst, B., Lodge, M., & Lavine, H. (2010). The attractiveness halo: Why some candidates are perceived more favorably than others. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 111–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 260–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Weston, E. M., Friday, A. E., & Lio, P. (2007). Biometric evidence that sexual selection has shaped the hominin face. PloS One, 2(8), e710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wilson, R. K., & Eckel, C. C. (2006). Judging a book by its cover: Beauty and expectations in the trust game. Political Research Quarterly, 59(2), 189–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wilson, J. P., & Rule, N. O. (2015). Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1325–1331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wincenciak, J., Dzhelyova, M., Perrett, D. I., & Barraclough, N. E. (2013). Adaption to facial trustworthiness is different in female and male observers. Vision Research, 87, 30–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O'Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5(3), 277–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2008). Friend or foe: The effect of implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision making. Cognition, 108, 796–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2004). Brisbane adolescent twin study: Outline of study methods and research projects. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56(2), 65–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zaatari, D., & Trivers, R. (2007). Fluctuating asymmetry and behavior in the ultimatum game in Jamaica. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(4), 223–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zaidel, D. W., Bava, S., & Reis, V. A. (2003). Relationship between facial asymmetry and judging trustworthiness in faces. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 8(3), 225–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Zebrowitz, L. A., Voinescu, L., & Collins, M. A. (1996). "Wide-eyed" and "crooked-faced": Determinants of perceived and real honesty across the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12), 1258–1269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anthony J. Lee
    • 1
  • Margaret J. Wright
    • 2
  • Nicholas G. Martin
    • 3
  • Matthew C. Keller
    • 4
    • 5
  • Brendan P. Zietsch
    • 3
    • 6
  1. 1.Institute of Neuroscience and PsychologyUniversity of GlasgowGlasgowUK
  2. 2.Queensland Brain InstituteBrisbaneAustralia
  3. 3.QIMR Berghofer Medical Research InstituteBrisbaneAustralia
  4. 4.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceUniversity of Colorado BoulderBoulderUSA
  5. 5.Institute for Behavioral GeneticsUniversity of Colorado BoulderBoulderUSA
  6. 6.School of PsychologyUniversity of QueenslandBrisbaneAustralia

Personalised recommendations