Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 67, Issue 4, pp 519–536 | Cite as

Investigating Relational Framing of Categorization in Young Children

  • Teresa Mulhern
  • Ian StewartEmail author
  • John Mc Elwee
Original Article

Abstract

The aims of the current study were to measure patterns of relational framing linked with categorization in young, typically developing children and to correlate framing performance with linguistic and cognitive potential as measured by standardized instruments, including the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT–4), the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales—Fifth Edition (SB5), and the Children’s Category Test (CCT). The relational protocol developed for this study assessed properties of relational framing in 3 relational domains, including nonarbitrary and arbitrary containment relations and arbitrary hierarchical relations. There were 50 participants, 10 from each of the following age ranges: 3–4, 4–5, 5–6, 6–7, and 7–8. The results provided data concerning the acquisition of relational categorization skills across childhood and also showed strong correlations between relational performance and that on each of the 3 standardized measures. The results are discussed in relation to previous research and for their implications in regard to future studies on relational framing and categorization in children.

Keywords

Relational frame theory Hierarchy Containment Intelligence Language Classification 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

This research was supported by a scholarship awarded to Teresa Mulhern. On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there are no conflicts of interest. This research was conducted at the National University of Ireland, Galway, and was supported by the National University of Ireland, Galway, Doctoral Research Scholarship Scheme, which was awarded to Teresa Mulhern.

References

  1. Arterberry, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2012). Categorization of real and replica objects by 14- and 18-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 606–612.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.04.007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Arterberry, M. E., & Kellman, P. J. (2016). Development of perception in infancy: The cradle of knowledge revisited. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Astley, S. L., Peissig, J. J., & Wasserman, E. A. (2001). Superordinate categorization via learned stimulus equivalence: Quantity of reinforcement, hedonic value, and the nature of the mediator. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 252–268.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.27.3.252.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Blewitt, P. (1989). Categorical hierarchies: Levels of knowledge and skill. Genetic Epistemologist, 17, 21–29.Google Scholar
  5. Blewitt, P. (1994). Understanding categorical hierarchies: The earliest levels of skill. Child Development, 65, 1279–1298.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00817.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blewitt, P., Michnick Golinkoff, R., & Alioto, A. (2000). Do toddlers have label preferences? A possible explanation for word refusals. First Language, 20, 253–272.  https://doi.org/10.1177/014272370002006002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boll, T. (1993). Children’s Category Test. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  8. Bornstein, M. H., & Arterberry, M. E. (2010). The development of object categorization in young children: Hierarchical inclusiveness, age, perceptual attribute, and group versus individual analyses. Developmental Psychology, 46, 350–365.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018411.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Carneiro, P., Albuquerque, P., & Fernandez, A. (2009). Opposite developmental trends for false recognition of basic and superordinate names. Memory, 17, 411–427.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902758847.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cassidy, S., Roche, B., Colbert, D., Stewart, I., & Grey, I. M. (2016). A relational frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 222–235.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61, 173–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Community-University Partnership for the Study of Children, Youth, and Families. (2011). Review of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT-4). Edmonton: Author.Google Scholar
  13. Deneault, J., & Ricard, M. (2005). The effect of hierarchical levels of categories on children’s deductive inferences about inclusion. International Journal of Psychology, 40, 65–79.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590444000032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Deneault, J., & Ricard, M. (2006). The assessment of children’s understanding of inclusion relations: Transitivity, asymmetry, and quantification. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 551–570.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0704_6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVT-4). Minneapolis: Pearson.Google Scholar
  16. Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2013). Advances in relational frame theory: Research and application. Oakland: Context Press.Google Scholar
  17. Eimas, P. D., & Quinn, P. C. (1994). Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. Child Development, 65, 903–917.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1131427.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Eimas, P. D., Quinn, P. C., & Cowan, P. (1994). Development of exclusivity in perceptually based categories of young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 418–431.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1994.1043.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Gil, E., Luciano, C., Ruiz, F. J., & Valdivia-Salas, S. (2012). A preliminary demonstration of transformation of functions through hierarchical relations. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 12, 1–19.Google Scholar
  20. Gore, N. J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10, 1–17.Google Scholar
  21. Greene, T. R. (1994). What kindergartners know about class containment hierarchies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 72–88.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1994.1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hajibayova, L. (2013). Basic-level categories: A review. Journal of Information Science, 39, 676–687.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0165551513481443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hayes, J., & Stewart, I. (2016). Comparing the effects of derived relational training and computer coding on intellectual potential in school-age children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 397–411.  https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12114.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001a). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  25. Hayes, S. C., Fox, E., Gifford, E. V., Wilson, K. G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001b). Derived relational responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 21–50). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  26. Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 207–227.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-207.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Luciano, C., Gòmez Becarra, I., & Rodríguez Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple-exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 349–365.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.08-06.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. MacNeill Horton, A. (1996). Book and test reviews. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 171–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Manders, K., & Hall, D. G. (2002). Comparison, basic-level categories, and the teaching of adjectives. Journal of Child Language, 29, 923–937.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005391.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Margolis, E., & Laurence, S. (2000). Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  31. Markman, E. M., & Seibert, J. (1976). Classes and collections: Internal organization and resulting holistic properties. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 561–577.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(76)90019-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mompeán, J. A. (2006). The phoneme as a basic-level category: Experimental evidence from English. International Journal of English Studies, 6, 141–172.Google Scholar
  33. Moran, L., Walsh, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2015). Correlating derived relational responding with linguistic and cognitive ability in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 32–43.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Murphy, G. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  35. O’Hora, D., Peláez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58, 569–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O’Connor, J., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: Extending the remit of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 1–17.Google Scholar
  37. Palmer, D. C. (2002). Psychological essentialism: A review of E. Margolis and S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: Core Readings. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 597–607.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.78-597.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of number. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  39. Quinn, P. C. (2016). Establishing cognitive organization in infancy: From perceptual grouping of objects to social classification of faces. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues, 3rd ed (pp. 79–104). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  40. Quinn, P. C., & Eimas, P. D. (1996). Perceptual cues that permit categorical differentiation of animal species by infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 189–211.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0047.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fifth Edition. Itasca: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
  42. Rostad, K., Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2012). Development of categorization in infancy: Advancing forward to the animate/inanimate level. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 584–595.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.05.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross-domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 369–385.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Slattery, B., & Stewart, I. (2014). Hierarchical classification as relational framing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 61–75.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.63.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Slattery, B., Stewart, I., & O’Hora, D. (2011). Testing for transitive class containment as a feature of hierarchical classification. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 243–260.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-243.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Song, L., Pruden, S. M., Michnick Golinkoff, R., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2016). Prelinguistic foundations of verb learning: Infants discriminate and categorize dynamic human actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 151, 77–95.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.004.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Stewart, I. (2016). The fruits of a functional approach for psychological science. International Journal of Psychology, 51, 15–27.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12184.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2009). Relational responding and conditional discrimination procedures: An apparent inconsistency and clarification. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 309–317.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Stewart, I., & Roche, B. (2013). Relational frame theory: An overview. In S. Dymond & B. Roche (Eds.), Advances in relational frame theory: Research and application (pp. 51–71). Oakland: Context Press.Google Scholar
  50. Thomas, H., & Horton, J. J. (1997). Competency criteria and the class inclusion task: Modeling judgements and justifications. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1060–1073.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.1060.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Valentin, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2012). Olfactory categorization: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 337–352.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.05.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Viscaíno-Torres, R. M., Ruiz, F. J., Luciano, C., López-López, J. C., Barbero-Rubio, A., & Gil, E. (2015). The effect of relational training on intelligence quotient: A case study. Psicothema: Revista Anual de Psicología, 27, 120–127.  https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.149.Google Scholar
  53. Wechsler, D. (1989). Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  54. Wechsler, D. (1991). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  55. Wechsler, D. (1997). The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS–III). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  56. Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2). Circle Pines: Pearson.Google Scholar
  57. Zentall, T. R., Galizio, M., & Critchfield, T. S. (2002). Categorization, concept learning, and behavior analysis: An introduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 237–248.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.78-237.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyNational University of Ireland GalwayGalwayIreland
  2. 2.Department of Special EducationWilkes UniversityWilkes-BarreUSA

Personalised recommendations