Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 64, Issue 4, pp 755–768 | Cite as

Competing Arbitrary and Non-Arbitrary Relational Responding in Normally Developing Children and Children Diagnosed with Autism

  • Neil KennyEmail author
  • Dermot Barnes-Holmes
  • Ian Stewart
Original Article

Abstract

The current study seeks to further investigate the previously reported disruptive effect of competing non-arbitrary stimulus relations on derived relational responding (Stewart et al. The Psychological Record, 52, 77–88, 2002; Kenny et al. The Psychological Record, 2014). Initially, Experiment 1 utilised procedures adapted from the previous Stewart et al. (The Psychological Record, 52, 77–88, 2002) study, rendering them developmentally appropriate for a participant population of normally developing children. The results showed all participants demonstrated equivalence class formation when only black stimuli were used and maintained criterion levels of equivalence-consistent responding in the Colour Test condition, where a competing non-arbitrary colour relation was present. Experiment 2 exposed a cohort of children diagnosed with autism to identical training and testing procedures. While these participants also demonstrated equivalence class formation with stimuli only black in colour, five of the six participants showed significantly lower levels of equivalence-consistent responding in the Colour Test condition. These results are consistent with previously reported research findings that children with autism perform more poorly in tasks containing competing sources of stimulus control (Huizinga et al. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036. 2006; Baker et al. Clinical Neurospsychologist, 15(3), 309–313. 2001; Pennington et al. 1997). The results of Experiment 1 suggest possible procedures to undermine spurious sources of non-arbitrary stimulus control in normally developing children.

Keywords

Autism Arbitrary Relational Responding Relational Frame Theory 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th Ed.) Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  2. Augustson, K. G., & Dougher, M. J. (1991). Teaching conditional discrimination to young children: some methodological successes and failures. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 9, 21–24.Google Scholar
  3. Baker, M. J., Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1998). Increasing the social behavior of young children with autism using their obsessive behaviors. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 23(4), 300–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baker, K., Segalowitz, S. J., & Ferlisi, M. C. (2001). The effect of differing scoring methods for the tower of London task on developmental patterns of performance. Clinical Neurospsychologist, 15(3), 309–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barnes, D., Browne, M., Smeets, P. M., & Roche, B. (1995a). A transfer of functions and a conditional transfer of functions through equivalence relations in three to six year old children. The Psychological Record, 45, 405–430.Google Scholar
  6. Barnes, D., Lawlor, H., Smeets, P. M., & Roche, B. (1995b). Stimulus equivalence and academic self-concept among mildly mentally handicapped and nonhandicapped children. The Psychological Record, 46, 87–107.Google Scholar
  7. Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Exemplar training and a derived transformation of functions in accordance with symmetry. The Psychological Record, 51, 287–308.Google Scholar
  8. Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., Strand, P., & Friman, P. (2004). Establishing relational responding in accordance with more-than and less-than as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 4, 531–558.Google Scholar
  9. Bennetto, L., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 40, 45–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Biro, S., & Russell, J. (2001). The execution of arbitrary procedures by children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 97–110.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Chavez-Brown, M., Scott, J., & Ross, D. E. (2005). Antecedent selection: comparing simplified and typical verbal antecedents for children with autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 14(3), 153–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  14. Dymond, S., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Schenk, J. (2005). Non-autyomated procedures in derived stimulus relations: a methodological note. The Psychological Record, 55, 461–481.Google Scholar
  15. Eikeseth, S., & Smith, T. (1992). The development of functional and equivalence classes in high-functioning autistic children: the role of naming. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 123–133.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Etzel, B. C., Milla, S. R., & Nicholas, M. D. (1996). Arranging the development of conceptual behavior: A technology for stimulus control. In S. W. Bijou & E. Ribes (Eds.), New directions in behavior development (pp. 91–130). Reno: Context Press.Google Scholar
  17. Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gorham, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Berens, N. M. (2009). Derived comparative and transitive relations in young children with and without autism. The Psychological Record, 59, 221–246.Google Scholar
  19. Greer, R. D., McCorkle, N. P., & Twyman, J. S. (1996). Preschool Inventory of Repertoires for Kindergarten (P.I.R.K.). Yonkers: The Fred S. Keller School and CABAS.Google Scholar
  20. Hayes, S. C., Gifford, E., & Ruckstuhl, L. E. (1996). Relational frame theory and a behavioural approach to executive function. In R. Lyon (Ed.), Attention, memory, and executive function. Brookes: Baltimore.Google Scholar
  21. Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in Autism. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Holth, P., & Arntzen, E. (1998). Stimulus familiarity and the delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence or consistent non-equivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 81–110.Google Scholar
  23. Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477–492.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & Van der Molen, M. V. (2006). Age related trends in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kenny, N., Devlin, S., et al. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2014). Competing arbitrary and non-arbitrary stimulus relations: The effect of exemplar training in adult participant. The Psychological Record. doi: 10.1007/s40732-014-0004-4.
  26. Lehto, J. H. (2004). A test of children’s goal directed behaviour: a pilot study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98(1), 223–236.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Levin, H. S., Goldstein, F. C., Williams, D. H., & Eisenberg, H. M. (1991a). The contribution of frontal lobe lesions to behavioural outcomes of closed head injuries. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 318–338). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Levin, L. H., Culhane, K. A., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A. J., Harward, H., et al. (1991b). Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(3), 377–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lipkens, G., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitudinal study of derived stimulus relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201–239.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Luciano, C., Becerra, I. G., & Valverde, M. R. (2007). The role of multiple exemplar training and naming in the establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 87, 349–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Maurice, C., Green, G., & Luce, R. S. (Eds.). (1996). Behavioral interventions for young children with autism. Austin: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  32. McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 562–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Kledaras, J. B., Iennaco, F. M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1990). Teaching relational discrimination to individuals with mental retardation: some problems and possible solutions. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 95, 283–296.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Murphy, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2005). Derived manding in children with autism: synthesizing Skinner’s Verbal Behavior with relational frame theory. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 445–462.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. O'Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). The role of verbal behavior, stimulus nameability, and familiarity on the equivalence performances of autistic and normally-developing children. The Psychological Record, 59, 53–74.Google Scholar
  36. O'Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O'Connor, J., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: extending the remit of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9, 1–17.Google Scholar
  37. Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171–177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. J., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081–1105.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive function and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87. Roberts & Pennington, 1996.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Pennington, B. F., Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., Griffith, E. M., Reed, D. T., & Shyu, V. (1997). Validity test of the executive dysfunction hypothesis of autism. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 143–178). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Pilgim, C., Jackson, J., & Galizio, M. (2000). Acquisition of arbitrary consitional discriminations by young normally developing children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 73(2), 177–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Quill, K. A. (1995). Teaching children with autism: Strategies to enhance communication and socialization. New York: Delmar.Google Scholar
  44. Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1989). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded adults: the effect of training the component simple discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 1–12.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1990). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded adults: the development of generalized skills. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 239–250.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1993). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded subjects: programming acquisition and learning set. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 571–585.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Schilmoeller, G. L., Schilmoeller, K. J., Etzel, B. C., & LeBlanc, J. M. (1979). Conditional discrimination responding after errorless and trial-and-error training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 405–420.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behaviour: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative, Inc.Google Scholar
  50. Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146.Google Scholar
  51. Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., Mundy, P., & Sherman, T. (1987). Cognition in autistic children. In D. J. Cohen & A. M. Donnellan (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 103–120). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  52. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). Stimulus equivalence and non-arbitrary relations. The Psychological Record, 52, 77–88.Google Scholar
  54. Taber, T. A., Alberto, P. A., & Fredrick, L. D. (1998). Use of self-operated auditory prompts by workers with moderate mental retardation to transition independently through vocational tasks. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 19, 327–345.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Turner, M. (1999). Repetitive behaviour in autism: a review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 839–849.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.Google Scholar
  57. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Grossier, D. B. (1991). A normative developmental study of executive function; A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(2), 131–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 11–29.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. World Health Organisation. (1992). The ICD-10 classification for mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
  60. Zygmont, D. M., Lazar, R. M., Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1992). Teaching arbitrary matching via sample stimulus-control shaping to young children and mentally retarded individuals: a methodological note. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 109–117.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of LimerickLimerickRepublic of Ireland
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, National University of IrelandMaynoothIreland
  3. 3.Department of Psychology, National University of IrelandGalwayIreland

Personalised recommendations