Biological Theory

, Volume 7, Issue 3, pp 227–236 | Cite as

The Evolutionary Rationale for Consciousness

Long Article

Abstract

To answer the question of why we have consciousness, I propose the following evolutionary trajectory leading to this feature: Nervous systems appeared for the purpose of orchestrating behavior. As a rule of thumb the challenges facing an animal concern either approach or avoidance. These two options were originally hard-wired as reflexes. Improvements in adaptability of response came with an expansion of the computational aspect of the system and a concomitant shift from simple reflexes to instinctual behavior, learning, and eventually, feelings. The assessment of positive and negative feelings allows organisms to weigh various options, but for this to be a viable strategy, an awareness of hedonic value is required. This was presumably the first neural attribute to evolve that required awareness, and thus the key force in the evolution of consciousness. The attribute first appeared in the early amniotes (the phylogenetic group comprising reptiles, birds and mammals). Support for this model in current accounts of the neurobiology of feelings and consciousness is discussed.

Keywords

Amniotes Consciousness Emotions Evolution Mood modules Self-awareness 

References

  1. Alkire MT, Miller J (2005) General anesthesia and the neural correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 150:229–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G (2008) Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322:876–880CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baars B (1997) In the theather of consciousness. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barbano MF, Cador M (2007) Opioids for hedonic experience and dopamine to get ready for it. Psychopharm 191:497–506CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumeister RF, Masicampo EJ, Vohs KD (2011) Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Ann Rev Psych 62:331–361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML (2011) Building a neuroscience of pleasure and well-being. Psych Well-Being 1:3Google Scholar
  7. Beshkar M (2008) The presence of consciousness in the absence of the cerebral cortex. Synapse 62:553–556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bittar RG, Kar-Purkayastha I, Owen SL, Bear RE, Green A, Wang S, Aziz TZ (2005) Deep brain stimulation for pain relief: a meta-analysis. J Clin Neurosci 12:515–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Borrelli L, Fiorito G (2008) Behavioral analysis of learning and memory in cephalopods. In: Menzel R (ed) Learning theory and behavior. Elsevier, New York, pp 605–628Google Scholar
  10. Butler AB (2008) Evolution of brains, cognition, and consciousness. Brain Res Bul 75:442–449CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Butler AB, Cotterill RM (2006) Mammalian and avian neuroanatomy and the question of consciousness in birds. Biol Bul 211:106–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buzsaki G (2007) The structure of consciousness. Nature 446:267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cabanac M (1992) Pleasure: the common currency. J Theor Biol 155:173–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cabanac M, Bonniot-Cabanac MC (2007) Decision making: rational or hedonic? Beh Brain Funct 3:45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cabanac M, Cabanac AJ, Parent A (2009) The emergence of consciousness in phylogeny. Beh Brain Res 198:267–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Crick F, Koch C (2003) A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 6:119–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Crick F, Koch C (2005) What is the function of the claustrum? Phil Trans Royal Soc London B 360:1271–1279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Damasio A (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Farrell M, Egan GF (2009) The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Conscious Cogn 18:500–514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Edelman G (2004) Wider than the sky: the phenomenal gift of consciousness. Yale University Press, YaleGoogle Scholar
  21. Edelman DB, Seth AK (2009) Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach. Trends Neurosc 32:476–484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ferrarelli F, Tononi G (2011) The thalamic reticular nucleus and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bul 37:306–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fiorito G, Scotto P (1992) Observational learning in octopus vulgaris. Science 256:545–547CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gould JL, Grant-Gould C (1995) The honey bee. Scientific American Library, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  25. Grinde B (2012) The biology of happiness. Springer, DordrechtCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gvilia I (2010) Underlying brain mechanisms that regulate sleep-wakefulness cycles. Int Rev Neurobio 93:1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hochner B, Shomrat T, Fiorito G (2006) The octopus: a model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms. Biol Bul 210:308–317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jones EG (2001) The thalamic matrix and thalamocortical synchrony. Trends Neurosc 24:595–601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kitchen A, Denton D, Brent L (1996) Self-recognition and abstraction abilities in the common chimpanzee studied with distorting mirrors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:7405–7408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC (2009) Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:479–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kunde W, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2003) Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition 88:223–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Laland KN, Galef BG (2009) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, HarvardGoogle Scholar
  33. Land M, Nilsson D-E (2002) Animal eyes. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  34. Lane N (2009) Life ascending: the ten great inventions of evolution. WW Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Leknes S, Tracey I (2008) A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nature Rev Neurosc 9:314–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI (2009) Neuroscience pains and pleasures of social life. Science 323:890–891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lou HC (2011) Paradigm shift in consciousness research: the child’s self-awareness and abnormalities in autism, ADHD and schizophrenia. Acta Paediat 101:112–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Marino L (2007) Cetacean brains: how aquatic are they? Anat Rec 290:694–700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mather JA (2008) Cephalopod consciousness: behavioural evidence. Conscious Cogn 17:37–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. McFarland DJ, Sibly RM (1975) The behavioural final common path. Philos Trans Royal Soc London B 270:265–293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Merker B (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Beh Brain Sci 30:63–81Google Scholar
  42. Miller JW, Ferrendelli JA (1990) Characterization of GABAergic seizure regulation in the midline thalamus. Neuropharma 29:649–655CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Mink JW, Blumenschine RJ, Adams DB (1981) Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Phys 241:R203–R212Google Scholar
  44. Moriyama T, Gunji YP (1997) Autonomous learning in maze solution by octopus. Ethology 103:499–513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mosley C (2011) Pain and nociception in reptiles. Vet Clin N Am Exotic Ani Prac 14:45–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nir Y, Tononi G (2010) Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends Cogn Sci 14:88–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Noirhomme Q, Soddu A, Lehembre R, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Boveroux P, Boly M, Laureys S (2010) Brain connectivity in pathological and pharmacological coma. Front Syst Neurosci 4:160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Palmiter RD (2011) Dopamine signaling as a neural correlate of consciousness. Neurosci 198:213–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Panksepp J (1998) Affective neuroscience. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  50. Panksepp J, Normansell L, Cox JF, Siviy SM (1994) Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Phys Behav 56:429–443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pecina S (2008) Opioid reward “liking” and “wanting” in the nucleus accumbens. Phys Behav 94:675–680Google Scholar
  52. Pessiglione M, Petrovic P, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2008) Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 59:561–567CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Prior H, Schwarz A, Gunturkun O (2008) Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biol 6:e202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Reiss D, Marino L (2001) Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5937–5942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Rose JD (2007) Anthropomorphism and “mental welfare” of fishes. Dis Aqua Org 75:139–154Google Scholar
  56. Rosenthal DM (2008) Consciousness and its function. Neuropsychologia 46:829–840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rossano MJ (2003) Expertise and the evolution of consciousness. Cognition 89:207–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Schiff ND, Fins JJ (2007) Deep brain stimulation and cognition: moving from animal to patient. Cur Opin Neurol 20:638–642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Seth AK, Baars BJ, Edelman DB (2005) Criteria for consciousness in humans and other mammals. Conscious Cogn 14:119–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Smith CU (2010) Darwin’s unsolved problem: the place of consciousness in an evolutionary world. J Hist Neurosci 19:105–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Smith K, Mahler S, Pecina S, Berridge K (2010) Hedonic hotspots: generating sensory pleasure in the brain. In: Kringelbach M, Berridge K (eds) Pleasures in the brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 27–49Google Scholar
  62. Sneddon LU (2009) Pain perception in fish: indicators and endpoints. ILAR J 50:338–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Szymusiak R, McGinty D (2008) Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and arousal. Ann New York Acad Sci 1129:275–286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (2008) The sunny side of fairness—preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychol Sci 19:339–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Takahashi H, Kato M, Matsuura M, Mobbs D, Suhara T, Okubo Y (2009) When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science 323:937–939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tamietto M, de Gelder B (2010) Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:697–709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Ward LM (2011) The thalamic dynamic core theory of conscious experience. Conscious Cogn 20:464–486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Zhang Y, Lu H, Bargmann CI (2005) Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 438:179–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Division of Mental HealthNorwegian Institute of Public HealthOsloNorway

Personalised recommendations