Demography

, Volume 51, Issue 2, pp 619–643 | Cite as

Does Education Improve Cognitive Performance Four Decades After School Completion?

  • Nicole Schneeweis
  • Vegard Skirbekk
  • Rudolf Winter-Ebmer
Article

Abstract

We study the effect of secondary education on cognitive performance toward the end of working age. We exploit the exogenous variation in years of schooling arising from compulsory schooling reforms implemented in six European countries during the 1950s and 1960s. Using data of individuals, approximately age 60, from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE), we assess the causal effect of education on memory, fluency, numeracy, and orientation-to-date. Furthermore, we study education effects on cognitive decline. We find a positive impact of schooling on memory scores. One year of education increases the memory score approximately four decades later by about 0.2, which amounts to 10 % of a standard deviation. Furthermore, we find some evidence for a protective effect of schooling on cognitive decline in terms of verbal fluency.

Keywords

Compulsory education Cognitive functioning Memory Aging 

References

  1. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (2010). Preventing Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline (AHRQ Publication No. 10-E005). Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.Google Scholar
  2. Angrist, J. D., Imbens, G. W., & Rubin, D. B. (1996). Identification of causal effects using instrumental variables. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 91, 444–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anstey, K. J., Lipnicki, D. M., & Low, L.-F. (2008). Cholesterol as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: A systematic review of prospective studies with meta-analysis. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 343–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anstey, K. J., von Sanden, C., Salim, A., & O’Kearney, R. (2007). Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. American Journal of Epidemiology, 166, 367–378.Google Scholar
  5. Banks, J., & Mazzonna, F. (2012). The effect of education on old age cognitive abilities: Evidence from a regression discontinuity design. Economic Journal, 122, 418–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bingley, P., & Martinello, A. (2011). Retirement improves cognitive performance (Working Paper No. 07). Copenhagen, Denmark: Danish National Center for Social Research.Google Scholar
  7. Black, S. E., Devereux, P. J., & Salvanes, K. G. (2008). Staying in the classroom and out of the maternity ward? The effect of compulsory schooling laws on teenage births. Economic Journal, 118, 1025–1054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonsang, E., Adam, S., & Perelman, S. (2012). Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? Journal of Health Economics, 31, 490–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brinch, C. N., & Galloway, T. A. (2012). Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109, 425–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brookmeyer, R., Johnson, E., Ziegler-Graham, K., & Arrighi, H. M. (2007). Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association, 3, 186–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brunello, G., Fabbri, D., & Fort, M. (2013). The causal effect of education on the body mass: Evidence from Europe. Journal of Labor Economics, 31, 195–223.Google Scholar
  12. Brunello, G., Fort, M., Schneeweis, N., & Winter-Ebmer, R. (2011). The causal effect of education on health: What’s the role of health behaviors? (IZA Discussion Paper No. 5944). Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Labor.Google Scholar
  13. Brunello, G., Fort, M., & Weber, G. (2009). Changes in compulsory schooling, education and the distribution of wages in Europe. Economic Journal, 119, 516–539.Google Scholar
  14. Cagney, K., & Lauderdale, D. (2002). Education, wealth and cognitive function in later life. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, 163–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cascio, E. U., & Lewis, E. G. (2006). Schooling and the armed forces qualifying test. Evidence from school-entry laws. Journal of Human Resources, 41, 294–318.Google Scholar
  16. Deary, I., Whiteman, M., Starr, J., Whalley, L., & Fox, H. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Engelhardt, H., Buber, I., Skirbekk, V., & Prskawetz, A. (2010). Social involvement, behavioural risks and cognitive functioning among older people. Ageing and Society, 30, 779–809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Falch, T., & Massih, S. (2011). The effect of education on cognitive ability. Economic Inquiry, 49, 838–856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fort, M., Schneeweis, N., & Winter-Ebmer, R. (2011). More schooling, more children: Compulsory schooling reforms and fertility in Europe (IZA Discussion Paper No. 6015). Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Labor.Google Scholar
  20. Garrouste, C. (2010). 100 years of educational reforms in Europe: A contextual database. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.Google Scholar
  21. Glymour, M. M., Kawachi, I., Jencks, C. S., & Berkman, L. F. (2008). Does childhood schooling affect old age memory or mental status? Using state schooling laws as natural experiments. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 62, 532–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hakansson, K., Rovio, S., Helkala, E.-L., Vilska, A.-R., Winblad, B., Soininen, H., . . . Kivipelto, M. (2009). Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: Population based cohort study. British Medical Journal, 339, 1–8.Google Scholar
  23. Haworth, C. M. A., Daleb, P., & Plomin, R. (2008). A twin study into the genetic and environmental influences on academic performance in science in nine-year-old boys and girls. International Journal of Science Education, 30, 1003–1025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Husén, T., & Tuijnman, A. (1991). The contribution of formal schooling to the increase in intellectual capital. Educational Researcher, 20, 17–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43, 13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lindenberger, U., & Ghisletta, P. (2009). Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: Gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychology and Aging, 24, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mårtensson, J., Eriksson, J., Bodammer, N. C., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Nyberg, L., & Lövdén, M. (2012). Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage, 63, 240–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Maurer, J. (2011). Education and male-female differences in later-life cognition: International evidence from Latin America and the Caribbean. Demography, 48, 915–930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mura, T., Dartigues, J.-F., & Berr, C. (2010). How many dementia cases in France and Europe? Alternative projections and scenarios 2010–2050. European Journal of Neurology, 17, 252–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Neisser, U., Boodoo, B., Bouchard, T., Boykin, A., Brody, B., Ceci, S., . . . Sternberg, S. (1997). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. In M. E. Hertzig & E. A. Faber (Eds.), Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development (pp. 95–134). Philadelphia, PA: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  31. Ninomiya, T., Ohara, T., Hirakawa, Y., Yoshida, D., Doi, Y., Hata, J., . . . Kiyohara, Y. (2011). Midlife and late-life blood pressure and dementia in Japanese elderly: The Hisayama study. Hypertension, 58, 22–28.Google Scholar
  32. Nisbett, R. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count. New York, NY: WW Norton.Google Scholar
  33. Nisbett, R., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. OECD. (2006). Live longer, work longer. Ageing and Employment Policies. Paris, France: Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Potter, G. G., Helms, M. J., & Plassman, B. L. (2008). Associations of job demands and intelligence with cognitive performance among men in late life. American Journal of Epidemiology, 70, 1803–1808.Google Scholar
  36. Ravaglia, G., Forti, P., Lucicesare, A., Pisacane, N., Rietti, E., Bianchin, M., & Dalmonte, E. (2008). Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: Findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology, 70, 1786–1794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Richards, M., & Hatch, S. L. (2011). A life course approach to the development of mental skills. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B(Suppl. 1), i26–i35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Romeu Gordo, L., & Skirbekk, V. (2013). Skill demand and the comparative advantage of age: Jobs tasks and earnings from the 1980s to the 2000s in Germany. Labour Economics, 22, 61–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Salthouse, T. (2010). Major issues in cognitive aging (Oxford Psychology Series). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Schmidt, F., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Schooler, C., Mulatu, M., & Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work in the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sharrett, R. A. (2012). Is cognitive aging predicted by educational level? American Journal of Epidemiology, 175, 760–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Skirbekk, V., Loichinger, E., & Weber, D. (2012). Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 770–774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Snowdon, D. A., & Kemper, S. J. (1996). Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Findings from the nun study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 528–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Thorvaldsson, V., Hofer, S. M., Berg, S., & Johansson, B. (2006). Effects of repeated testing in a longitudinal age-homogeneous study of cognitive aging. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, 348–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. United Nations. (2011). World population prospects. New York, NY: United Nations Population Division.Google Scholar
  47. Warr, P. (1994). Age and employment. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 485–550). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.Google Scholar
  48. Whalley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal, 322, 819. doi:10.1136/bmj.322.7290.819 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Xu, W. L., Atti, A. R., Gatz, M., Pedersen, N. L., Johannson, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2011). Midlife overweight and obesity increase late-life dementia risk: A population-based twin study. Neurology, 76, 1568–1574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Yang, Y.-H., Roe, C. M., & Morris, J. C. (2011). Relationship between late-life hypertension, blood pressure, and Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 26, 457–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Yount, K. M. (2008). Gender, resources across the life cycle, and cognitive functioning in Egypt. Demography, 45, 907–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Population Association of America 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicole Schneeweis
    • 1
    • 2
  • Vegard Skirbekk
    • 3
  • Rudolf Winter-Ebmer
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.Department of EconomicsJohannes Kepler University LinzLinzAustria
  2. 2.IZABonnGermany
  3. 3.IIASALaxenburgAustria
  4. 4.IHSViennaAustria
  5. 5.CEPRLondonUK

Personalised recommendations