Skip to main content
Log in

Replacing Epiphenomenalism: a Pluralistic Enactive Take on the Metaplasticity of Early Body Ornamentation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Philosophy & Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the domain of evolutionary cognitive archaeology, the early body ornaments from the Middle Stone Age/Palaeolithic are generally treated as mere by-products of an evolved brain-bound cognitive architecture selected to cope with looming social problems. Such adaptive artefacts are therefore taken to have been but passive means of broadcasting a priori envisaged meanings, essentially playing a neutral role for the human mind. In contrast to this epiphenomenalist view of material culture, postphenomenology and the Material Engagement Theory (MET) have been making a case for the active role of artefacts on the count that they can actually shape and restructure the human mind. By bringing these dissenting voices together, the paper at hand employs an enactive way of thinking in order to challenge the epiphenomenalist take on early body ornaments. In fact, two variants of enactivism are presented, each advancing a unique explanation of how the engagement of early humans with body ornaments transformed their minds along the two postphenomenological categories of embodied and hermeneutic cognition. Our theoretical frameworks specifically seek to explore how early beadworks could have scaffolded the creation of semiotic categories and the development of cognitive processes. Despite relying on inherently different premises, both theories suggest that beads fostered the emergence of an epistemic apparatus which thoroughly transformed the way humans engaged with the world. Having concurred on the ornaments’ transformative effects, we ultimately conclude that the epiphenomenalist paradigm best be replaced with an enactive approach grounded on the dictates of postphenomenology and the MET.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. At this point, it should be acknowledged that few evolutionary psychology supporters currently share these deterministic positions, since most would attempt to reconcile the evolution of a fixed mental architecture with the variability emerging from the ethnographic analyses of multiple human cultures. In this light, a possible escape from the criticism of fostering “deterministic drifts” lies in the idea that artefacts change their form according to the context where they belong. Regardless, the relationship between the human mind and technology, in this milder version of evolutionary psychology, would still boil down to superficial variations in the form assumed by material culture, which were ultimately modelled onto an ancestral world (see Ingold 2004, and Nash 2014, for a critique of the environmental triggering of pre-specified functions).

  2. The term non-utilitarian denotes their communicative function and is used in contradistinction to the term utilitarian, which refers to non-significative artefacts and practices that had been used for the purposes of subsistence and settlement (e.g. Bednarik 1992; Bednarik 1995; d’Errico et al. 2003; d’Errico and Nowell 2000; Henshilwood and d’Errico 2011; Marshack 1989; Mellars 1989).

  3. According to Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011, p.390), higher theory of mind pertains to the understanding of false beliefs and other abstract mental states (e.g. higher-order desires) and differs from the capacity of apes and young children to read behavioural intentions.

  4. As Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011, p.367) see it, level-2 perspective-taking pertains to the ability to inhibit one’s own perspective and comprehend how an object looks from another person’s perspective.

  5. Adhering to scholarly tradition, Peirce’s work is cited as CP (followed by volume and paragraph number in The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce).

  6. While sign function (i.e. taking something to stand for something else) is of a conceptual nature, appreciating iconic and indexical relevance is largely a matter of perception (albeit a more-or-less conceptually influenced one). The meaning found at this level is yielded dialectically at the crux between matter and mind. While the physical world presents its various qualities, the mind only brings some to the forefront in order to experience a thematic take of its environment. Given that each interpreter focuses on different aspects of the physical world (due to their sensory system constraints, as well as prior experience), the meaning of a material sign emerges as a selective interpretation. To this extent, the meaning of a material sign is not specified solely in the structure between—let’s say—a footprint and the animal that left it behind (as maintained by REC), but in the habit of interpretation yielded from the understanding of their indexical relevance. To this extent, the footprint is not understood by way of imaginative re-enactment (as REC would have had it)—at least not initially. One must, in fact, first grasp the indexical relevance of spatiotemporally contiguity between footprint and animal, before being able to re-imagine anything specific. If we combine the fact about selective interpretation, with the fact that sign function is a concept that specifically concerns how something stands for something else, we can come to appreciate why, unlike REC, the pragmatic and enactive theory of cognitive semiotics is not quick to remove from the study of material signification the notion of re-presentation (i.e. the repeated and thematic presentation to the mind of a seemingly factual thing or event). It should be underlined here that in following the tradition of Peircean semiotics, this representation is seen as one of a mediative kind, as the Representamen (or Sign) actually mediates the meaning of its Object to its Interpretant.

  7. The idea that contentful signs can be established only through a system of public representations and norms partly resonates with Deacon’s (1997) idea that full symbols can exist only within systems of symbols, and not in isolation from them (cf. Sonesson 2006).

  8. See Garofoli (2017a) for specific discussion of ornament-mediated cognitive transformation in Neanderthals.

References

  • Abadía, O. M., & Nowell, A. (2015). Palaeolithic personal ornaments: historical development and epistemological challenges. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 22(3), 952–979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, P. J. (2010). From executive mechanisms underlying perception and action to the parallel processing of meaning. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S39–S54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Yosef, O. (2002). The Upper Paleolithic revolution. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31(1), 363–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. C. (2013). The archaeology of mind: it’s not what you think. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(01), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Bertone, C., & Castiello, U. (2008). How the gaze of others influences object processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 254–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarik, R. G. (1992). Palaeoart and archaeological myths. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 2(1), 27–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarik, R. G. (1995). Concept-mediated marking in the Lower Paleolithic. Current Anthropology, 36(4), 605–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., & Smith, E. A. (2005). Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology, 46, 221–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouzouggar, A., Barton, N., Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., Collcutt, S., Higham, T., et al. (2007). 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(24), 9964–9969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bueno, O. (2013). Perception and conception: shaping human minds. Biosemiotics, 6(3), 323–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. (2012). Evolutionary psychology: the new science of the mind. London: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, P. G. (1994). On symbols and the Paleolithic. Current Anthropology, 35(5), 627–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J. (2015). Hominin language development: a new method of archaeological assessment. Biosemiotics, 8(1), 67–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J. (2017). Accessing hominin cognition: language and social signaling in the Lower to Middle Palaeolithic. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Paleolithic archaeology (pp. 157–195). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard, N. J. (2010). Cultural modernity: consensus or conundrum? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(17), 7621–7622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: new perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 201–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Banks, W. E. (2013). Identifying mechanisms behind Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age cultural trajectories. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S371–S387.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A.-M., Soressi, M., et al. (2003). Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music—an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory, 17(1), 1–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M., & van Niekerk, K. (2005). Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(1), 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Henshilwood, C. S. (2011). The origin of symbolically mediated behaviour: from antagonistic scenarios to a unified research strategy. In C. S. Henshilwood & F. d’Errico (Eds.), Homo symbolicus: the dawn of language imagination and spirituality (pp. 49–73). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Nowell, A. (2000). A new look at the Berekhat Ram figurine: implications for the origins of symbolism. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 10(1), 123–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Vanhaeren, M. (2009). Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origin of language debate. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of human language (pp. 16–40). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D., et al. (2009). Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(38), 16051–16056.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, M. (2009). Theory of mind: how children understand others’ thoughts and feelings. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dretske, F. I. (1986). Misrepresentation. In R. Bogdan (Ed.), Belief: form, content, and function (pp. 17–36). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). Theory of mind and the evolution of language. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language (pp. 92–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness. Cambridge: Massachusetts MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenici, M. (2012). Embodied social cognition and embedded theory of mind. Biolinguistics, 6(3–4), 276–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiebich, A. (2016). Narratives, culture, and folk psychology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 135–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flom, R., & Johnson, S. (2011). The effects of adults’ affective expression and direction of visual gaze on 12-month-olds’ visual preferences for an object following a 5-minute, 1-day, or 1-month delay. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 64–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Froese, T., Lenay, C., & Ikegami, T. (2012). Imitation by social interaction? Analysis of a minimal agent-based model of the correspondence problem. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(202).

  • Gallagher, S. (2008). Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 535–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, C. (1998). Palaeolithic society and the release from proximity: a network approach to intimate relations. World Archaeology, 29, 426–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, C., Gowlett, J., & Dunbar, R. (2011). The social brain and the shape of the Palaeolithic. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 21(1), 115–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garofoli, D. (2015). Do early body ornaments prove cognitive modernity? A critical analysis from situated cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 803–825.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garofoli, D. (2016). Metaplasticit-ies: material engagement meets mutational enhancement. In G. Etzelmüller & C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment in evolution and culture (pp. 307–335). Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garofoli, D. (2017a). Ornamental feathers without mentalism: a radical enactive view on Neanderthal body adornment. In C. Durt, T. Fuchs, & C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment, enaction, culture: investigating the constitution of the shared world (pp. 279–306). Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

  • Garofoli, D. (2017b). RECkoning with representational apriorism in evolutionary cognitive archaeology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9549-4.

  • Goeminne, G., & Paredis, E. (2011). Opening up the in-between: Ihde’s postphenomenology and beyond. Foundations of Science, 16(2–3), 101–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett, J., Gamble, C., & Dunbar, R. (2012). Human evolution and the archaeology of the social brain. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 693–722.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidle, M. N., Garofoli, D., Scheiffele, S., & Stolarczyk, R. (Submitted). Fooling around with a piece of scoria—inventions, innovations, and their relation to human cultural evolution. In S. Hansen, F. Klimscha, & J. Renn (Eds.), Contextualising ancient innovations. Berlin: TOPOI Edition.

  • Harrold, F. B. (2009). Historical perspectives on the European transition from Middle to Upper Paleolithic. In M. Camps & P. Chauhan (Eds.), Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions (pp. 283–299). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 11(2), 58–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962 [1927]). Being and time. New York: Harper & Row.

  • Henshilwood, C. S. (2007). Fully symbolic sapiens behaviour: innovation in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. In C. Stringer & P. Mellars (Eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins and dispersal of modern humans (pp. 123–132). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S. (2009). The origins of symbolism, spirituality and shamans: exploring Middle Stone Age material culture in South Africa. In C. Renfrew & I. Morley (Eds.), Becoming human: innovation in prehistoric material and spiritual cultures (pp. 29–49). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K., & Jacobs, Z. (2004). Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science, 304(5669), 404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., d'Errico, F., & Watts, I. (2009). Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 57(1), 27–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., & d’Errico, F. (2011). Middle Stone Age engravings and their significance to the debate on the emergence of symbolic material culture. In C. S. Henshilwood & F. d’Errico (Eds.), Homo symbolicus: the dawn of language imagination and spirituality (pp. 75–96). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2009). Reading the artefacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 41–61). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2011). The Still Bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka: symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 52(3), 361–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., & Marean, C. W. (2003). The origin of modern human behavior: critique of the models and their test implications. Current Anthropology, 44(5), 627–651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2007). Semiotic scaffolding in living systems. In M. Barbieri (Ed.), Introduction to biosemiotics: the new biological synthesis (pp. 149–166). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2014a). The semiome: from genetic to semiotic scaffolding. Semiotica, 198, 11–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2014b). Semiotic scaffolding: a biosemiotic link between sema and soma. In K. R. Cabell & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The catalyzing mind (pp. 95–110). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1555–1577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2008a). First communions: mimetic sharing without theory of mind. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity (Vol. 12, pp. 245–276). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D. (2008b). Folk psychological narratives: the sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. (2013). Exorcising action oriented representations: ridding cognitive science of its Nazgul. Adaptive Behavior, 21(3), 142–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: basic minds without content. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

  • Hutto, D. D. (2015a). Basic social cognition without mindreading: minding minds without attributing contents. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0831-0.

  • Hutto, D. D. (2015b). Overly enactive imagination? Radically re-imagining imagining. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 53(S1), 68–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (forthcoming). Going radical. In A. Newen, L. de Bruin & S. Gallagher (Eds.), The oxford handbook of 4E cognition.

  • Ihde, D. (1990). Technology and the lifeworld: from garden to earth. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2002). Bodies in technology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2008). Introduction: postphenomenological research. Human Studies, 31(1), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2009). Postphenomenology and technoscience. the Peking University lectures. Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2010). Embodied technics. New York: Automatic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iliopoulos, A. (2015). The prehistory of material signification: tracing the nature and emergence of early body ornamentation through a pragmatic and enactive theory of cognitive semiotics. D.Phil. Thesis, University of Oxford. https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:0811d8f8-e885-4785-b7a6-681faaceca41.

  • Iliopoulos, A. (2016a). The evolution of material signification: tracing the origins of symbolic body ornamentation through a pragmatic and enactive theory of cognitive semiotics. Signs and Society, 4(2), 244–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iliopoulos, A. (2016b). The material dimensions of signification: rethinking the nature and emergence of semiosis in the debate on human origins. Quaternary International, 405(Part A), 111–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iliopoulos, A., & Garofoli, D. (2016). The material dimensions of cognition: reexamining the nature and emergence of the human mind. Quaternary International, 405(Part A), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2004). Beyond biology and culture. The meaning of evolution in a relational world. Social Anthropology, 12(2), 209–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2007). The trouble with ‘evolutionary biology’. Anthropology Today, 23(2), 13–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions, revised edition: genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press.

  • Jaspers, K. (1953). The origin and goal of history. Translated by Michael Bullock. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaspers, K. (1963). The atom bomb and the future of man. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, R. A. (2005). Archaeology of the body. Annual Reviews of Anthropology, 34, 139–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. G. (2000). Archeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(1), 17–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. G. (2009). The human career (3rd ed.). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. (2012). Emergent patterns of creativity and innovation in early technologies. In S. Elias (Ed.), Origins of human innovation and creativity (pp. 69–87). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L. (2014). Signaling theory and technologies of communication in the Paleolithic. Biological Theory, 9, 42–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2007a). Body ornamentation as information technology: towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In P. Mellars, K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef, & C. Stringer (Eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins and dispersal of modern humans (pp. 45–54). Cambridge: McDonald Institute of Archaeology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2007b). Paleolithic ornaments: implications for cognition, demography and identity. Diogenes, 54(2), 40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L., Stiner, M. C., Reese, D. S., & Güleç, E. (2001). Ornaments of the earliest Upper Paleolithic: new insights from the Levant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(13), 7641–7646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & O’Brien, M. J. (2010). Niche construction theory and archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17(4), 303–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., et al. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813), 20151019.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1994). On technical mediation. Common Knowledge, 3(2), 29–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lele, V. P. (2006). Material habits, identity, semeiotic. Journal of Social Archaeology, 6(1), 48–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenay, C., & Stewart, J. (2012). Minimalist approach to perceptual interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 528–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. (2012). Thinking through the Middle Stone Age of sub-Saharan Africa. Quaternary International, 270, 140–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2007). Before and beyond representation: towards an enactive conception of the Palaeolithic image. In C. Renfrew & I. Morley (Eds.), Image and imagination: a global history of figurative representation (pp. 287–300). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2008). Beads for a plastic mind: the ‘Blind Man’s Stick’ (BMS) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18(3), 401–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2010). Metaplasticity and the human becoming: principles of neuroarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 88, 49–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2011). Comment on ‘The Still Bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka: symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 52(3), 385–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: a theory of material engagement. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), 351–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. S., & Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism-I: how the brain constructs cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshack, A. (1989). Evolution of the human capacity: the symbolic evidence. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 32, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGann, M. (2014). Enacting a social ecology: radically embodied intersubjectivity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1321). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01321.

  • Mellars, P. (1989). Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. Current Anthropology, 30(3), 349–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellars, P. (2006). Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(25), 9381–9386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R., & Kirchhoff, M. (2014). Cognitive transformations and extended expertise. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 46(6), 610–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miłkowski, M. (2015). The hard problem of content: solved (long ago). Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric, 41(1), 73–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: the cognitive origins of art and science. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (2005). The singing Neanderthals: the origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, A. (2014). Are stone-age genes created out of whole cloth? Evaluating claims about the evolution of behavior. Dialectical Anthropology, 38(3), 305–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, A. (2010). Defining behavioral modernity in the context of Neandertal and anatomically modern human populations. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39(1), 437–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(05), 939–973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Origgi, G., & Sperber, D. (2000). Evolution, communication and the proper function of language. In P. Carruthers & A. Chamberlain (Eds.), Evolution and the human mind: language, modularity and social cognition (pp. 140–169). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann, K. A. (2013). Material scaffolds in numbers and time. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(1), 19–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann, K. A. (2016). The role of materiality in numerical cognition. Quaternary International, 405(Part A), 42–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S. (2000). The ontogeny of information: developmental systems and evolution. Durham (NC): Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1873). On the nature of signs. In C. J. W. Kloesel (Ed.), Writings of Charles Sanders Peirce: a chronological edition, Vol. 3 (1872–1878) (pp. 66–68). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1931–1935). The collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce: In C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss (Eds. vols. 1–6), & A. W. Burks (Ed. vols. 7–8). Cambridge: Harvard University Press [cited as CP].

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. W. W. Norton & Company.

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978). When is attribution of beliefs justified?[P&W]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 592–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberger, R., & Verbeek, P.-P. (Eds.). (2015). Postphenomenological investigations: essays on human-technology relations. Lanham (MD): Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Modularity, development and ‘theory of mind’. Mind & Language, 14(1), 131–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soffer, O. (2009). Defining modernity, establishing rubicons, imagining the other—and the Neanderthal enigma. In M. Camps & P. Chauhan (Eds.), Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions (pp. 43–64). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soffer, O., Adovasio, J. M., & Hyland, D. C. (2000). The “Venus” figurines: textiles, basketry, gender, and status in the Upper Paleolithic. Current Anthropology, 41(4), 511–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (1989a). Pictorial concepts: inquiries into the semiotic heritage and its relevance for the analysis of the visual world. Lund: Lund University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (1989b). Semiotics of photography—on tracing the index. Lund: Lund University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (2006). The meaning of meaning in biology and cognitive science: a semiotic reconstruction. Sign System Studies, 34(1), 135–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (2007). From the meaning of embodiment to the embodiment of meanings: a study in phenomenological semiotics. In T. Ziemke, J. Zlatev, & R. Frank (Eds.), Body, language, and mind, Embodiment (Vol. 1, pp. 85–128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (2010). From mimicry to mime by way of mimesis: reflections on a general theory of iconicity. Sign System Studies, 38, 18–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (2012). Semiosis beyond signs. On two or three missing links on the way to human beings. In T. Schilhab, F. Stjernfelt, & T. Deacon (Eds.), The symbolic species evolved (pp. 81–93). Dordrecht: Springer.

  • Sonesson, G. (2013a). The natural history of branching: approaches to the phenomenology of firstness, secondness, and thirdness. Signs and Society, 1(2), 297–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonesson, G. (2013b). The picture between mirror and mind: from phenomenology to empirical studies in pictorial semiotics. In K. Sachs-Hombach & J. R. J. Schirra (Eds.), Origins of pictures: anthropological discourses in image science (pp. 270–310). Cologne: Herbert von Halem.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: the evolution of human cognition. New York: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2012). The evolved apprentice: how evolution made humans unique. Cambridge (MA): MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, M. C. (2014). Finding a common band-width: causes of convergence and diversity in Paleolithic beads. Biological Theory, 9, 51–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stotz, K. (2010). Human nature and cognitive–developmental niche construction. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 483–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stotz, K. (2014). Extended evolutionary psychology: the importance of transgenerational developmental plasticity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 908.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallis, R. (2011). Aping mankind: neuromania, Darwinitis and the misrepresentation of humanity. Durham: Acumen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulloa, J. L., Marchetti, C., Taffou, M., & George, N. (2014). Only your eyes tell me what you like: exploring the liking effect induced by other’s gaze. Cognition and Emotion, 29(3), 460–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren, M., & d’Errico, F. (2006). Aurignacian ethno-linguistic geography of Europe revealed by personal ornaments. Journal of Archaeological Science, 33(8), 1105–1128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., van Niekerk, K. L., Henshilwood, C. S., & Erasmus, R. M. (2013). Thinking strings: additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 64(6), 500–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, P.-P. (2005). What things do: philosophical reflections on technology, agency, and design. University Park (PA): Penn State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L. (2001). What is cultural modernity? A general view and a South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(2), 201–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, T. (2016). The cultural dimension of cognition. Quaternary International, 405(Part A), 91–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M., & Clark, A. (2008). Culture, embodiment and genes: unravelling the triple helix. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3563–3575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2007). Did a small but significant enhancement in working memory capacity power the evolution of modern thinking? In P. Mellars, K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef, & C. B. Stringer (Eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans (pp. 79–90). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T., Overmann, K. A., Coolidge, F. L., & Janulis, K. (2017). Bootstrapping ordinal thinking. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Palaeolithic archaeology (pp. 197–213). New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahidi, K., & Myin, E. (2016). Radically enactive numerical cognition. In G. Etzelmüller & C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment in evolution and culture (pp. 57–71). Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: an archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioral modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15(1), 1–54.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the editors Prof. Luciano Floridi, Prof. Don Ihde, and in particular Dr. Lambros Malafouris for their assistance during the submission and review process. We would also like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments provided on an earlier version of this manuscript. Duilio Garofoli is funded by the Gerda Henkel Foundation and has provided the artwork for Fig. 1.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Duilio Garofoli.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Garofoli, D., Iliopoulos, A. Replacing Epiphenomenalism: a Pluralistic Enactive Take on the Metaplasticity of Early Body Ornamentation. Philos. Technol. 32, 215–242 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-017-0296-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-017-0296-9

Keywords

Navigation