Advertisement

Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 6, Issue 1, pp 99–116 | Cite as

The Cognitive Design of Tools of Thought

  • Barbara Tversky
Article

Abstract

When thought overwhelms the mind, the mind puts it into the world, notably in diagrams and gestures.Both use space and arrays of elements, depictive and non-depictive, to convey ideas, concrete and abstract,clear and sketchy. The arrays and the non-depictive elements like boxes and arrows serve to showrelationships and organizations, thematic, categorical, and more. on paper, in the air, in the diagrammedworld. Human actions organize space to convey abstractions: spraction.

Keywords

Mental Rotation Perceptual Inference Expert Designer External Representation Visual Explanation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

I am indebted to my colleagues and collaborators, especially Valeria Giardino, Jocelyn Penny Small, Azadeh Jamalian, Angela Kessell, Julie Heiser, Paul Lee, and Jeff Zacks. I am grateful the Varieties of Understanding Project at Fordham University and The John Templeton Foundation and to the following National Science Foundation grants for facilitating the research and/or preparing the manuscript: National Science Foundation HHC 0905417, IIS-0725223, IIS-0855995, and REC 0440103. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the funders.

References

  1. Azema, M., and F. Rivere. 2012. Animation in Paleolithic art: A pre-echo of cinema. Antiquity 86: 316–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bender, J., and M. Marrinan. 2010. The culture of diagram. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bilda, Z and Gero, J.S. (2006) Reasoning with internal and external representations: A case study with expert architects. In R Sun and N. Miyake (Editors), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum, Pp. 1020–1026.Google Scholar
  4. Boone, E. H. (2010). Stories in Red and Black: Pictorial Histories of the Aztec and Mixtec. University of Texas PressGoogle Scholar
  5. Brew, K. and Guerra, R. (2013). Design is One: Lella and Massimo Vignelli. Film. US distributor www.firstrunfeatures.com
  6. Brown, L. 1979. The story of maps. NY: Dover.Google Scholar
  7. Casati, R., and A. Varzi. 1996. Introduction. In Events, ed. R. Casati and A. Varzi. Aldershot: Dartmouth.Google Scholar
  8. Chu, M., and S. Kita. 2008. Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137: 706–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Denis, M. 1997. The description of routes: A cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 16: 409–458.Google Scholar
  10. Denis, M. Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C., & Bertolo, L. (1998). Spatial discourse and navigation: An analysis of route directions in the city of Venice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12 Google Scholar
  11. Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Egoff, D.B., and L.E. Corder. 1991. Height differences of low and high job status, female and male corporate employees. Sex Roles 24: 365–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Emmorey, K., B. Tversky, and H. Taylor. 2000. Using space to describe space: Perspective in speech, sign, and gesture. Spatial Cognition and Computation 2: 157–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Enfield, N.J. 2003. Producing and editing diagrams using co-speech gesture: Spatializing non-spatial relations in explanations of kinship in Laos. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 13(1): 7–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goodman, N.A. 1976. Languages of art: An approach to a theory of symbols. Indianopolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
  16. Heiser, J., and B. Tversky. 2006. Arrows in comprehending and producing mechanical diagrams. Cognitive Science 30: 581–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Heiser, J., B. Tversky, and M. Silverman. 2004. Sketches for and from collaboration. In Visual and spatial reasoning in design III, ed. J.S. Gero, B. Tversky, and T. Knight, 69–78. Sydney: Key Centre for Design Research.Google Scholar
  18. Hutchins, E. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Jamalian, A., and B. Tversky. 2012. Gestures alter thinking about time. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, and R.P. Cooper, 551–557. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  20. Jamalian, A., V. Giardino, and B. Tversky. 2013. Gestures for thinking. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sabaenz, and I. Wachsmuth. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  21. Johnson, S. 2006. The ghost map. London: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
  22. Jurafsky, D. (2013). Why ice cream sounds fat and crackers sound skinny. Stanford Magazine, July/AugustGoogle Scholar
  23. Kang, S., B. Tversky, and J.B. Black. 2012. From hands to minds: How gestures promote action understanding. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, and R.P. Cooper, 551–557. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  24. Kang, S., Tversky, B. & Black, J. B. (2014) Gesture and speech in explanations to experts and novices.Google Scholar
  25. Kantrowitz, A. (2014). A cognitive ethnographic study of improvisational drawing by eight contemporary artists. Doctoral dissertation, Columbia Teachers CollegeGoogle Scholar
  26. Kirsh, D. 1995. The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence 73: 31–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Malafouris, L. 2013. How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. McCloud, S. 1993. Understanding comics. Northhampton: Kitchen Sink.Google Scholar
  29. Miller, G.A., and P.N. Johnson-Laird. 1976. Language and perception. NY: Belnap Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Netz, R. 1999. Linguistic formulae as cognitive tools. Pragmatics and Cognition 7: 147–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Norman, D.A. 1993. Things that make us smart. Boston: Addison-Wesley Longman.Google Scholar
  32. Pike, A.W.G., D.L. Hoffmann, M. Garcia-Diez, P.B. Pettitt, J. Alcolea, R. De Balb, C. Gonzalez-Sainz, C. de las Heras, J.A. Lasheras, R. Motes, and J. Zilhao. 2012. U-Series dating of Paleolithic art in 11 caves in Spain. Science 336: 1409–1413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sadier, B., J.-J. Delannoy, L. Benedetti, D. Bourles, S. Jaillet, J.-M. Geneste, A.-E. Lebatard, and M. Arnold. 2012. Further constraints on the Chauvet cave artwork elaboration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 8002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schon, D.A. 1983. The reflective practitioner. NY: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  35. Schubert, T., and A. Maass (eds.). 2011. Spatial dimensions of social thought. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  36. Shepard, R.N. 1984. Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review 91: 417–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Small, J.P. 1999. Time in space: Narrative in classical art. Art Bulletin 81: 562–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Smith, S.M. 1995. Getting into and out of mental ruts: A theory of fixation, incubation, and insight. In The nature of insight, ed. R.J. Sternberg and J.E. Davidson, 229–251. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  39. Stjernfelt, F. 2011. Diagrammatology: An investigation on the borderlines of phenomenology, ontology, and semiotics. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
  40. Suchman, L. 1987. Plans and situated actions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 1996. What architects see in their sketches: Implications for design tools. Human factors in computing systems: Conference companion (pp. 191–192). NY: ACM.Google Scholar
  42. Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 2001. Constructive perception in design. In Computational and cognitive models of creative design V, ed. J.S. Gero and M.L. Maher, 227–239. Sydney: University of Sydney.Google Scholar
  43. Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 2003. Constructive perception: A skill for coordinating perception and conception. In Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Meetings, ed. R. Alterman and D. Kirsh. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  44. Suwa, M., B. Tversky, J. Gero, and T. Purcell. 2001. Seeing into sketches: Regrouping parts encourages new interpretations. In Visual and spatial reasoning in design, ed. J.S. Gero, B. Tversky, and T. Purcell, 207–219. Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition.Google Scholar
  45. Talmy, L. 1983. How language structures space. In Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application, ed. H.L. Pick Jr. and L.P. Acredolo, 225–282. N. Y.: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics. Vols 1 & 2. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  47. Tversky, B. 1981. Distortions in memory for maps. Cognitive Psychology 13: 407–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tversky, B. 1990. Where partonomies and taxonomies meet. In Meanings and prototypes: Studies on linguistic categorization, ed. S.L. Tsohatzidis, 334–344. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  49. Tversky, B. 1995. Cognitive origins of graphic conventions. In Understanding images, ed. F.T. Marchese, 29–53. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Tversky, B. 2001. Spatial schemas in depictions. In Spatial schemas and abstract thought, ed. M. Gattis, 79–111. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  51. Tversky, B. 2004. Semantics, syntax, and pragmatics of graphics. In Language and visualisation, ed. K. Holmqvist and Y. Ericsson, 141–158. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Tversky, B. 2005. How to get around by mind and body: Spatial thought, spatial action. In Cognition, evolution, and rationality: A cognitive science for the XXIst century, ed. A. Zilhao. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  53. Tversky, B. 2011a. Spatial thought, social thought. In Spatial dimensions of social thought, ed. T. Schubert and A. Maass. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  54. Tversky, B. 2011b. Visualizations of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science 3: 499–535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tversky, B., and K. Hemenway. 1983. Categories of scenes. Cognitive Psychology 15: 121–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tversky, B. and Kessell, A. (2014). Thinking in action. Topics in Cognitive Science. Google Scholar
  57. Tversky, B., and P.U. Lee. 1998. How space structures language. In Spatial Cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representation and processing of spatial knowledge, ed. C. Freksa, C. Habel, and K.F. Wender, 157–175. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tversky, B., and P.U. Lee. 1999. Pictorial and verbal tools for conveying routes. In Spatial information theory: cognitive and computational foundations of geographic information science, ed. C. Freksa and D.M. Mark, 51–64. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  59. Tversky, B., and M. Suwa. 2009. Thinking with sketches. In Tools for innovation, ed. A. Markman. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Tversky, B., and J.M. Zacks. 2013. Event perception. In Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology, ed. D. Riesberg. Oxford: Oxford.Google Scholar
  61. Tversky, B., S. Kugelmass, and A. Winter. 1991. Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology 23: 515–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tversky, B., J. Zacks, P.U. Lee, and J. Heiser. 2000. Lines, blobs, crosses, and arrows: Diagrammatic communication with schematic figures. In Theory and application of diagrams, ed. M. Anderson, P. Cheng, and V. Haarslev, 221–230. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tversky, B., J. Heiser, P. Lee, and M.-P. Daniel. 2009. Explanations in gesture, diagram, and word. In Spatial language and dialogue, ed. K.R. Coventry, T. Tenbrink, and J.A. Bateman, 119–131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tversky, B., J. Heiser, and J. Morrison. 2013. Space, time, and story. In The psychology of learning and motivation, ed. B.H. Ross, 47–76. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  65. Wexler, M., S.M. Kosslyn, and A. Berthoz. 1998. Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition 68: 77–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wohlschläger, A., and A. Wohlschläger. 1998. Mental and manual rotatation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24: 397–412.Google Scholar
  67. Zacks, J., and B. Tversky. 1999. Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication. Memory and Cognition 27: 1073–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Zacks, J., and B. Tversky. 2001. Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin 127: 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Columbia Teachers College and Stanford UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations