Skip to main content
Log in

Ready to Teach or Ready to Learn: A Critique of the Natural Pedagogy Theory

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

According to the theory of natural pedagogy, humans have a set of cognitive adaptations specialized for transmitting and receiving knowledge through teaching; young children can acquire generalizable knowledge from ostensive signals even in a single interaction, and adults also actively teach young children. In this article, we critically examine the theory and argue that ostensive signals do not always allow children to learn generalizable knowledge more efficiently, and that the empirical evidence provided in favor of the theory of natural pedagogy does not defend the theory as presented, nor does it support a weakened version of the theory. We argue that these problems arise because the theory of natural pedagogy is grounded in a misguided assumption, namely that learning about the world and learning about people are two distinct and independent processes. If, on the other hand, we see the processes as interrelated, then we have a better explanation for the empirical evidence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Moreover, the theory of natural pedagogy includes not only the learners’ side but also the teachers’ side, such that we should expect cognitive adaptations for teachers as well as learners (e.g., Gelman et al., 2013). However, since there is no clear empirical evidence that adults have the variety of cognitive adaptations predicted for teachers, our examination will focus on the learner’s side of the natural pedagogy theory.

  2. Only one exception is Hewlett et al. (2011)’s report showing that !Kung children depend largely on explicit teaching. However, if Csibra and Gergely’s evolutionary story is right, we should expect to find explicit teaching in many different cultures. This one exception is insufficient for supporting their claim.

  3. Note that Sterelny himself thinks his model is consistent with the theory of natural pedagogy. However, his argument challenges Csibra and Geregly’s evolutionary story for the theory of natural pedagogy by showing that the evolution of complex cultures does not necessarily need the evolution of teaching with ostensive communications.

  4. For instance, as we have already mentioned, they said “[i]nfants smile to any adult who communicates to them” not just their parents (Csibra & Gergely, 2011, 1155, italics added). They claim too that children can “learn generic kind-relevant information directly…from benevolent communicators who manifest generic knowledge ‘for’ them that would be difficult (if not impossible) to acquire without such support.” (Csibra & Gergely, 2009, 152). These are just two examples of the many instances in which Csibra and Gergely suggest that natural pedagogy allows children to gain generalizable knowledge from any adult who ostensively communicates with them.

  5. For the sake of space, the paper does not refer to some experiments on function of ostensive cues, especially on identity of objects (Yoon et al. 2008) and A-not-B errors (Topál et al. 2008) though it seems that both have some problems. For instance, Yoon et al. (2008) does not seem to distinguish between identifying an object over time and as a member of a kind, and Vorms (2012) has also pointed out some problems with Topál et al. (2008).

References

  • Andrews, K. 2012. Do apes read minds? Toward a new folk psychology. MIT Press.

  • Buttlemann, D., N. Zmyi, M. Daum, and M. Carpenter. 2013. Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development 84(2): 422–428.

  • Boch, J. 2005. What makes a competent adult forager? In Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental & cultural perspectives, ed. M. E. Lamb, B. S. Hewlett, 109–128. Transaction.

  • Caro, T.M., and M.D. Hauser. 1992. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review of Biology 67(2): 151–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chouinard, M.M. 2007. Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 72(1): 1–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., S. Heller, S. Biro, and J. Henrich. 2012. Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior 33(1): 46–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. 2010. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language 25(2): 141–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. and G. Gergely. 2006. Social learning and social cognition: the case for pedagogy. In Processes of change in brain and cognitive development (Attention and performance vol. 21), ed. Y. Munakata, M. H. Johnson, 249–274. Oxford University Press

  • Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4): 148–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 2011. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1149–1157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egyed, K., I. Király, and G. Gergely. 2013. Communicating shared knowledge without language in infancy. Psychological Science 24(7): 1348--1353.

  • Farroni, T., G. Csibra, F. Simion, and M.H. Johnson. 2002. Eye contact in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99(14): 9602–9605.

  • Feiring, C., M. Lewis, and M.D. Starr. 1984. Indirect effects and infants’ reaction to strangers. Developmental Psychology 20(3): 485–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Futó, J., E. Téglás, G. Csibra, and G. Gergely. 2010. Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition 117: 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S., E.A. Ware, E.M. Manczak, and S.A. Graham. 2013. Children’s sensitivity to the knowledge expressed in pedagogical and nonpedagogical contexts. Developmental Psychology 49(3): 491--504.

  • Gergely, G. 2008. Learning “about” versus learning “from” other minds. In The innate mind; Foundations and the future, ed. P. Carruthers, S. Lawrence, S. Stich, 170-198. Oxford University Press

  • Gergely, G., H. Bekkering, and I. Király. 2002. Rational imitation in preverbal infants: Babies may opt for a simpler way to turn on a light after watching an adult do it. Nature 755: 415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., and G. Csibra. 2005. The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies 6(3): 463–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., K. Egyed, and I. Király. 2007. On pedagogy. Developmental Science 10(1): 139–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.L., and K.H. Corriveau. 2011. Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1179–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and F. Gil-White. 2001. The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22(3): 165–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B., H. Fouts, A. Boyette, and B. Hewlett. 2011. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1168–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes, T. 1651/2011. Leviathan. Empire Books.

  • Hood, L. and L. Bloom. 1979. What, when, and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 44(6): 1--47.

  • Hume, D. 1739-40/1978. A treatise of human nature. Oxford University Press.

  • Keupp, S., T. Behne, and H. Rakoczy. 2013. Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116 (2): 392–406.

  • Király, I., G. Csibra, and G. Gergely. 2004. The role of communicative-referential cues in observational learning during the second year. Poster presented at the 14th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies. 2004. Chicago, IL: USA.

  • Király, I., G. Csibra, and G. Gergely. 2013. Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116(2): 471--486.

  • Klinnert, M.D. 1984. The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant Behavior & Development 7: 447–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, M.A., and P.L. Harris. 2005. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development 76(6): 1261–1277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D.E., A.G. Young, and F.C. Keil. 2007. The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences 104 (50): 19751--19756.

  • Lyons, D. E., D. H. Damrosch, J. K. Lin, D. M. Macris, and F. C. Keil. 2011. The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artifact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1158–1167.

  • MacDonald, K. 2007. Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting. Human Nature 18: 386–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F.W. 2005. Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 14: 54–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F.W. 2007. Hunting and gathering: The human sexual division of foraging labor. Cross-Cultural Research 41: 170–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 1988. Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology 24: 470–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 1996. The human infant as imitative generalist: A 20-year progress report on infant imitation with implications for comparative psychology. In Social learning in animals: The roots of culture, ed. C.M. Heyes and B.G. Galef, 347–370. New York: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K., and R. Baillargeon. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308: 255–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinkham, A.M., and V.K. Jaswal. 2011. Watch and learn? Infants privilege efficiency over pedagogy during imitative learning. Infancy 16(5): 535–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin-Dubois, D., I. Brooker, and A. Polonia. 2011. Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behavior and Development 34: 303–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repacholi, B.M., and A. Gopnik. 1997. Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology 33(1): 12–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sage, K.D., and D. Baldwin. 2011. Disentangling the social and the pedagogical in infants’ learning about tool-use. Social Development 20(4): 825–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M.F.H., H. Rakoczy, and M. Tomasello. 2010. Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science 14(3): 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., and G. Csibra. 2008. Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology 18(9): 668–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. 2012. The evolved apprentice: How evolution made humans unique. The MIT Press

  • Stenberg, G. 2012. Why do infants look at and use positive information from some informants rather than others in ambiguous situations? Infancy 17: 672–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., and N.J. Raihani. 2008. The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour 75: 1823–1836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., G. Gergely, Á. Miklóski, Á. Erdőhegyi, and G. Csibra, G. 2008. Infant’s perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science 321: 1831–1834.

  • Vorms, M. 2012. A-not-B errors: Testing the limits of natural pedagogy. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 3(4): 525–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walden, T. A. and T. A. Ogan. 1988. The development of social referencing. Child Development 59 (5): 1230–1240.

  • Wellman, H.M., D. Cross, and J. Watson. 2001. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child development 72(3): 655–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, J.H., D.F. Sacco, K. Hugenberg, and K.D. Williams. 2010. Eye gaze as relational evaluation: Averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(7): 869–882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, J.M.D., M.H. Johnson, and G. Csibra. 2008. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(36): 13690–13695.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grant-in-Aid for JSPS Fellows and Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas No. 26118504 “The Evolutionary Origin and Neural Basis for the Empathethic Systems” to the first author. We are grateful to Olivia Sultanescu for her helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hisashi Nakao.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nakao, H., Andrews, K. Ready to Teach or Ready to Learn: A Critique of the Natural Pedagogy Theory. Rev.Phil.Psych. 5, 465–483 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-014-0187-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-014-0187-2

Keywords

Navigation