Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 4, Issue 1, pp 107–133 | Cite as

The Cognitive Integration of E-Memory

Article

Abstract

If we are flexible, hybrid and unfinished creatures that tend to incorporate or at least employ technological artefacts in our cognitive lives, then the sort of technological regime we live under should shape the kinds of minds we possess and the sorts of beings we are. E-Memory consists in digital systems and services we use to record, store and access digital memory traces to augment, re-use or replace organismic systems of memory. I consider the various advantages of extended and embedded approaches to cognition in making sense of E-Memory and some of the problems that debate can engender. I also explore how the different approaches imply different answers to questions such as: does our use of internet technology imply the diminishment of ourselves and our cognitive abilities? Whether or not our technologies can become actual parts of our minds, they may still influence our cognitive profile. I suggest E-Memory systems have four factors: totality, practical cognitive incorporability, autonomy and entanglement which conjointly have a novel incorporation profile and hence afford some novel cognitive possibilities. I find that thanks to the properties of totality and incorporability we can expect an increasing reliance on E-Memory. Yet the potentially highly entangled and autonomous nature of these technologie pose questions about whether they should really be counted as proper parts of our minds.

References

  1. Adams, F., and K. Aizawa. 2001. The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 14(1): 43–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. 1992. Working memory: The interface between memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4(3): 281–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bell, C., and J. Gemmell. 2009. Total recall: How the E-memory revolution will change everything. Dutton.Google Scholar
  4. Berry, E., N. Kapur, L. Williams, S. Hodges, P. Watson, G. Smyth, et al. 2007. The use of a wearable camera, SenseCam, as a pictorial diary to improve autobiographical memory in a patient with limbic encephalitis: A preliminary report. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 17(4–5): 582–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Campbell, J. 2002. The ownership of thoughts. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology 9(1): 35–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carr, N. 2008. Is Google making us stupid? Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education 107(2): 89–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carr, N. 2010. The shallows: How the internet is changing the way we think, read and remember. London: Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
  8. Chalmers, D. 2007. Forward to supersizing the mind. In Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Clark, A. 2003. Natural born cyborgs: Minds, technologies and the future of human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark, A., and D. Chalmers. 1998. The extended mind. Analysis 58: 10–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clowes, R. W. 2012. Hybrid memory, cognitive technology and self. In ed. Y. Erdin and M. Bishop. Proceedings of AISB/IACAP World Congres.Google Scholar
  13. Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Donald, M. 1993. Precis of the origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 737–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gemmell, J., G. Bell, and R. Lueder. 2006. MyLifeBits: A personal database for everything. Communications of the ACM 49(1): 88–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Goody, J. 1977. The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Greenfield, S. 2008. ID: The quest for identity in the 21st century. London: Sceptre.Google Scholar
  18. Gregory, R.L. 1981. Mind in science: A history of explanations in psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Halverson, J. 1992. Goody and the implosion of the literacy thesis. Man 27(2): 301–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Heersmink, R. 2012. Mind and artifact: A multidimensional matrix for exploring cognition-artifact relations. In Proceedings of AISB/IACAP World Congres 2012.Google Scholar
  21. Heidegger, M. 1927. Being and time. Oxford: Basil, Blackwell.Google Scholar
  22. Hodges, S., Williams, L., Berry, E., Izadi, S., Srinivasan, J., Butler, A., et al. 2006. SenseCam: A retrospective memory aid. UbiComp 2006: Ubiquitous computing, 177–193.Google Scholar
  23. Hoerl, C. 2007. Episodic memory, autobiographical memory, narrative: On three key notions in current approaches to memory development. Philosophical Psychology 20(5): 621–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hutchins, E. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. Kalnikaite, V., and S. Whittaker 2008. Cueing digital memory: how and why do digital notes help us remember? Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 22nd British HCI Group Annual Conference on People and Computers: Culture, Creativity, Interaction-Volume 1.Google Scholar
  26. Kalnikaite, V., Sellen, A., Whittaker, S., and D. Kirk. 2010. Now let me see where I was: Understanding how lifelogs mediate memory. Paper Presented at the Proceedings of the 28th International Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems.Google Scholar
  27. Lanier, J. 2010. You are not a gadget: A manifesto. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
  28. Leadbeater, C. 2008. We-think: Mass innovation not mass production. Profile.Google Scholar
  29. Loftus, E.F., and G.R. Loftus. 1980. On the permanence of stored information in the human brain. American Psychologist 35(5): 409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Loftus, E.F., and J.C. Palmer. 1974. Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13(5): 585–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Luria, A.R., and L.S. Vygotsky. 1992. Ape, primitive man and child: Essays in the history of behaviour. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
  32. Marcus, G. 2008. What if HM had a Blackberry? Coping with amnesia, using modern technology. Psychology Today.Google Scholar
  33. Mayer-Schönberger, V. 2011. Delete: The virtue of forgetting in the digital age. Princeton: Princeton Univ.Google Scholar
  34. McLuhan, M. 2001 [1964]. Understanding media: The extensions of man. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  35. Menary, R. 2010. Cognitive integration and the extended mind. In The extended mind, ed. R. Menary, 227–244. London: Bradford Book, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Mithen, S. 1996. The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames Hudson.Google Scholar
  37. Nolan, C. 2000. Memento. Los Angeles: Newmarket Films.Google Scholar
  38. Norman, D.A. 1999. The invisible computer: Why good products can fail, the personal computer is so complex, and information appliances are the solution. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  39. Olson, D. 1994. The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Ong. 1982. Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. Methuen.Google Scholar
  41. Rowlands, M. 1999. The body in mind: Understanding cognitive processes. Cambridge: CUP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rupert, R.D. 2004. Challenges to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy 101: 389–428.Google Scholar
  43. Rupert, R.D. 2009. Cognitive systems and the extended mind. USA: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schacter, D.L. 1987. Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13(3): 501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sellen, A.J., and S. Whittaker. 2010. Beyond total capture: A constructive critique of lifelogging. Communications of the ACM 53(5): 70–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Simmons, D. 2010. Ilium. Gateway.Google Scholar
  47. Smart, P.R. 2012. The web-extended mind. Metaphilosophy 43(4): 446–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Smart, P. R., Engelbrecht, P., Braines, D., Strub, M., and J. Hendler. 2009. Cognitive extension and the web. Paper presented at the WebSci’09: Society, Athens, Greece.Google Scholar
  49. Sparrow, B., J. Liu, and D.M. Wegner. 2011. Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science 333(6043): 776–778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sutton, J. 2003. Constructive memory and distributed cognition: Towards an interdisciplinary framework. In Constructive Memory, ed. B. Kokinov and W. Hirst, 290–303.Google Scholar
  51. Sutton, J. 2010. Exograms and interdisciplinarity: history, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In The extended mind, ed. R. Menary, 189–225. London: Bradford Book, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  52. Tulving, E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In Organisation of memory, ed. E. Tulving and W. Donaldson. New York: New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  53. Turkle, S. 2011. Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from each other. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  54. Watson, S.J. 2011. Before I go to sleep. London: Random House.Google Scholar
  55. Wegner, D.M. 1987. Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In Theories of group behavior, ed. B. Mullen and G. R. Goethals, 185–208. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  56. Wilson, R.A., and A. Clark. 2009. How to situate cognition: Letting nature take its course. In The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition, ed. M. Aydede and P. Robbins, 55–77. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Yeo, R. 2008. Notebooks as memory aids: Precepts and practices in early modern England. Memory Studies 1(1): 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.IFL, Faculdade de Ciências Sociais e HumanasUniversidade Nova de LisboaLisboaPortugal

Personalised recommendations