Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 3, Issue 2, pp 187–212

In Defense of Phenomenological Approaches to Social Cognition: Interacting with the Critics

Article

Abstract

I clarify recently developed phenomenological approaches to social cognition. These are approaches that, drawing on developmental science, social neuroscience, and dynamic systems theory, emphasize the involvement of embodied and enactive processes together with communicative and narrative practices in contexts of intersubjective understanding. I review some of the evidence that supports these approaches. I consider a variety of criticisms leveled against them, and defend the role of phenomenology in the explanation of social cognition. Finally, I show how these phenomenological approaches can solve the “starting problem” of social cognition.

References

  1. Astington, J. 1996. What is theoretical about the child’s theory of mind? In Theories of theory of mind, ed. P. Carruthers and P. Smith, 184–199. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Baillargeon, R., R.M. Scott, and Z. He. 2010. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(3): 110–118.Google Scholar
  3. Baird, J.A., and D.A. Baldwin. 2001. Making sense of human behavior: Action parsing and intentional inference. In Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition, ed. B.F. Malle, L.J. Moses, and D.A. Baldwin, 193–206. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  4. Baldwin, D.A., and J.A. Baird. 2001. Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Science 5(4): 171–178.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. 1991. Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading, ed. A. Whiten, 233–251. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., and J. Swettenham. 1996. The relationship between SAM and ToMM: Two hypotheses. In Theories of theory of mind, ed. P. Carruthers and P. Smith, 158–168. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Beer, R.D. 2000. Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(3): 91–99.Google Scholar
  9. Bruner, J. 1986. Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Bruner, J., and D.A. Kalmar. 1998. Narrative and metanarrative in the construction of self. In Self-awareness: Its nature and development, ed. M. Ferrari and R.J. Sternberg, 308–331. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  11. Buttelmann, D., M. Carpenter, and M. Tomasello. 2009. Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition 112: 337–342.Google Scholar
  12. Caggiano, V., L. Fogassi, G. Rizzolatti, P. Thier, and A. Casile. 2009. Mirror neurons differentially encode the peripersonal and extrapersonal space of monkeys. Science 324: 403–406.Google Scholar
  13. Carruthers, P. 1996. Simulation and self-knowledge: A defence of the theory-theory. In Theories of theories of mind, ed. P. Carruthers and P.K. Smith, 22–38. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Carruthers, P. 2002. The cognitive functions of language. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 657–726.Google Scholar
  15. Carruthers, P. 2009. How we know our own minds: the relationship between mindreading and metacognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(2): 121–182.Google Scholar
  16. Catmur, C., V. Walsh, and C. Heyes. 2007. Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology 17(17): 1527–1531.Google Scholar
  17. Csibra, G. 2005. Mirror neurons and action observation. Is simulation involved? ESF Interdisciplines. http://www.interdisciplines.org/mirror/papers/.
  18. Csibra, G., and G. Gergely. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 148–153.Google Scholar
  19. Currie, G. 2008. Some ways of understanding people. Philosophical Explorations 11(3): 211–218.Google Scholar
  20. Davies, M., and T. Stone. 1998. Folk psychology and mental simulation. In Contemporary issues in the philosophy of mind, royal institute of philosophy, Supplement 42, ed. A. O’Hear. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. De Jaegher, H. and E. Di Paolo. 2007. Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 6: 485–507.Google Scholar
  22. De Jaegher, H., E. Di Paolo, and S. Gallagher. 2010. Does social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10): 441–447.Google Scholar
  23. Dinstein, I., C. Thomas, M. Behrmann, and D.J. Heeger. 2008. A mirror up to nature. Current Biology 18(1): R13–R18.Google Scholar
  24. Dittrich, W.H., T. Troscianko, S.E.G. Lea, and D. Morgan. 1996. Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception 25: 727–738.Google Scholar
  25. Dokic, J. 2002. Reply to Pierre Jacob. In Simulation and knowledge of action, ed. J. Dokic and J. Proust, 111–117. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  26. Froese, T., and E.A. Di Paolo. 2010. Modeling social interaction as perceptual crossing: an investigation into the dynamics of the interaction process. Connection Science 22(1): 43–68.Google Scholar
  27. Froese, T., and E.A. Di Paolo. 2011. Toward minimally social behavior: social psychology meets evolutionary robotics. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 5777(2011): 426–433.Google Scholar
  28. Froese, T., and S. Gallagher. 2010. Phenomenology and artificial life: toward a technological supplementation of phenomenological methodology. Husserl Studies 26(2): 83–106.Google Scholar
  29. Fuchs, T., and H. De Jaegher. 2009. Enactive intersubjectivity: participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8: 465–486.Google Scholar
  30. Gallagher, S. 2001. The practice of mind: theory, simulation or primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5–7): 83–108.Google Scholar
  31. Gallagher, S. 2004. Understanding problems in autism: interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 11: 199–217.Google Scholar
  32. Gallagher, S. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Gallagher, S. 2007. Simulation trouble. Social Neuroscience 2(3–4): 353–365.Google Scholar
  34. Gallagher, S. 2008. Inference or interaction: social cognition without precursors. Philosophical Explorations 11(3): 163–173.Google Scholar
  35. Gallagher, S. 2009. Two problems of intersubjectivity. Journal of Consciousness Studies 16(6–8): 289–308.Google Scholar
  36. Gallagher, S., and D. Hutto. 2008. Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, 17–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  37. Gallagher, S., and A.N. Meltzoff. 1996. The earliest sense of self and others. Philosophical Psychology 9: 213–236.Google Scholar
  38. Gallagher, S., and D. Zahavi. 2008. The phenomenological mind. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  39. Gallese, V. 2001. The ‘shared manifold’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 33–50.Google Scholar
  40. Gallese, V. 2005. ‘Being like me’: Self-other identity, mirror neurons and empathy. In Perspectives on imitation I, ed. S. Hurley and N. Chater, 101–118. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  41. Goldman, A.I. 1995. Desire, intention and the simulation theory. In Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition, ed. B.F. Malle, L.J. Moses, and D.A. Baldwin, 207–224. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  42. Goldman, A.I. 2005. Imitation, mind reading, and simulation. In Perspectives on imitation II, ed. S. Hurley and N. Chater, 80–91. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  43. Goldman, A.I. 2006. Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Goldman, A., and C.S. Sripada. 2005. Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition 94: 193–213.Google Scholar
  45. Gopnik, A., and A.N. Meltzoff. 1997. Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  46. Gopnik, A., A.N. Meltzoff, and P.K. Kuhl. 1999. The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind. New York: HarperCollins Publishers.Google Scholar
  47. Gordon, R.M. 1995. Developing commonsense psychology: Experimental data and philosophical data. Paper presented at the APA Eastern Division Symposium on Children’s Theory of Mind, 12/27/95. (http://www.umsl.edu/~philo/Mind_Seminar/New%20Pages/papers/Gordon/apakids9.htm)
  48. Harvey, I., E.A. Di Paolo, R. Wood, M. Quinn, and E.A. Tuci. 2005. Evolutionary robotics: a new scientific tool for studying cognition. Artificial Life 11(1–2): 79–98.Google Scholar
  49. Herschbach, M. 2007. The phenomenological critics of folk psychology: The case of false belief. Paper presented at the Berkeley-Stanford-Davis Graduate Student Conference, 14 April 2007.Google Scholar
  50. Herschbach, M. 2008. Folk psychological and phenomenological accounts of social perception. Philosophical Explorations 11: 223–235.Google Scholar
  51. Hobson, P. 1993. The emotional origins of social understanding. Philosophical Psychology 6: 227–249.Google Scholar
  52. Hobson, P. 2002. The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  53. Hobson, P., and A. Lee. 1999. Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40: 649–659.Google Scholar
  54. Howe, M.L. 2000. The fate of early memories: Developmental science and the retention of childhood experiences. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  55. Hurley, S.L. 2005. Active perception and perceiving action: The shared circuits model. In Perceptual experience, ed. T. Gendler and J. Hawthorne, 205–259. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Hutto, D.D. 2008. Folk psychological narratives: The socio-cultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Iacoboni, M., I. Molnar-Szakacs, V. Gallese, G. Buccino, J.C. Mazziotta, and G. Rizzolatti. 2005. Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology 3(3): 529–535.Google Scholar
  58. Iizuka, H., and E.A. Di Paolo. 2007. Minimal agency detection of embodied agents. In Advances in artificial life: Proc. of the 9th Euro. Conf. on Artificial Life, ed. F. Almeida e Costa, L.M. Rocha, E. Costa, I. Harvey, and A. Coutinho, 485–494. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  59. Issartel, J., L. Marin, and M. Cadopi. 2007. Unintended interpersonal coordination: ‘Can we march to the beat of our own drum’? Neuroscience Letters 411: 174–179.Google Scholar
  60. Jacob, P. 2011. The direct perception model of empathy: a critique. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 2: 519–540.Google Scholar
  61. Jeannerod, M., and E. Pacherie. 2004. Agency, simulation, and self-identification. Mind and Language 19(2): 113–146.Google Scholar
  62. Kendon, A. 1990. Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Keysers, C., and V. Gazzola. 2006. Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition. Progress in Brain Research 156: 383–406.Google Scholar
  64. Kovács, A.M., E. Téglás, and A.D. Endress. 2010. The social sense: susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science 330: 1830–1834.Google Scholar
  65. Lindblom, J. 2007. Minding the Body: Interacting socially through embodied action. Linköping Studies in Science and Technology. Dissertation No. 1112.Google Scholar
  66. Lindblom, J., and T. Ziemke. 2007. Embodiment and social interaction: implications for cognitive science. In Body, language, and mind: Embodiment, ed. T. Ziemke, J. Zlatev, and R. Frank, 129–162. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  67. Maurer, D., and M.E. Barrera. 1981. Infants’ perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. Child Development 52(1): 196–202.Google Scholar
  68. Meltzoff, A.N. 1995. Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 31: 838–850.Google Scholar
  69. Meltzoff, A., and M.K. Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78.Google Scholar
  70. Meltzoff, A., and M.K. Moore. 1994. Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior & Development 17: 83–99.Google Scholar
  71. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1962. Phenomenology of perception. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  72. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1964. In The primacy of perception, ed. J. M. Edie. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1968. The visible and the invisible. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  74. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1987. Signs. Evanston: Northwestern University Press. Trans. R. C. McCleary.Google Scholar
  75. Moll, H., and M. Tomasello. 2004. 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science 7: F1–F9.Google Scholar
  76. Murray, L., and C. Trevarthen. 1985. Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Social perception in infants, ed. T.M. Field and N.A. Fox, 177–197. Norwood: Alex.Google Scholar
  77. Nelson, K. 2003. Narrative and the emergence of a consciousness of Self. In Narrative and consciousness, ed. G.D. Fireman, T.E.J. McVay, and O. Flanagan. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  78. Newman-Norlund, R.D., M.L. Noordzij, R.G.J. Meulenbroek, and H. Bekkering. 2007. Exploring the brain basis of joint attention: co-ordination of actions, goals and intentions. Social Neuroscience 2(1): 48–65.Google Scholar
  79. Nichols, S., and S.P. Stich. 2003. Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  80. Onishi, K.H., and R. Baillargeon. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308: 255–258.Google Scholar
  81. Pacherie, E. 2008. The phenomenology of action: a conceptual framework. Cognition 107: 179–217.Google Scholar
  82. Perner, J., and T. Ruffman. 2005. Infants’ insight into the mind: how deep? Science 308: 214–216.Google Scholar
  83. Povinelli, D.J., and J. Vonk. 2003. Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(4): 157–160.Google Scholar
  84. Rakoczy, H., N. Brosche, F. Warneken, and M. Michael Tomasello. 2009. Young children’s understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 27: 445–456.Google Scholar
  85. Ratcliffe, M. 2007. Rethinking commonsense psychology: A critique of folk psychology, theory of mind and simulation. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  86. Reddy, V. 2008. How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 661–670.Google Scholar
  88. Ruffman, T., and J. Perner. 2005. Do infants really understand false belief? Response to Leslie. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(10): 462–463.Google Scholar
  89. Saxe, R. 2005. Against simulation: the argument from error. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(4): 174–179.Google Scholar
  90. Saxe, R., S. Carey, and N. Kanwisher. 2004. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology 55: 87–124.Google Scholar
  91. Searle, J. 1983. Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  92. Senju, A., M.H. Johnson, and G. Csibra. 2006. The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception. Social Neuroscience 1(3–4): 220–234.Google Scholar
  93. Simons, D.J., and C.F. Chabris. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28: 1059–1074.Google Scholar
  94. Song, H.-J., K.H. Onishi, R. Baillargeon, and C. Fisher. 2008. Can an agent’s false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition 109(3): 295–315.Google Scholar
  95. Southgate, V., A. Senju, and G. Csibra. 2007. Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science 18(7): 587–592.Google Scholar
  96. Southgate, V., C. Chevallier, and G. Csibra. 2010. Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental Science 13(6): 907–912.Google Scholar
  97. Spaulding, S. 2010. Embodied cognition and mindreading. Mind and Language 25(1): 119–140.Google Scholar
  98. Stack, J., and C. Lewis. 2008. Steering towards a developmental account of infant social understanding. Human Development 51: 229–234.Google Scholar
  99. Stich, S., and S. Nichols. 1992. Folk psychology: simulation or tacit theory? Mind & Language 7(1): 35–71.Google Scholar
  100. Stich, S., and S. Nichols. 1995. Second thoughts on simulation. In Mental simulation: Evaluations and applications, ed. M. Davies and T. Stone, 87–108. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  101. Surian, L., S. Caldi, and D. Sperber. 2007. Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science 18(7): 580–586.Google Scholar
  102. Träuble, B., V. Marinović, and S. Pauen. 2010. Early theory of mind competencies: do infants understand others’ beliefs? Infancy 15(4): 434–444.Google Scholar
  103. Trevarthen, C.B. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–48. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  104. Trevarthen, C., and P. Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language, ed. A. Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.Google Scholar
  105. Varela, F., E. Thompson, and E. Rosch. 1991. The embodied mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  106. Walker, A.S. 1982. Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33: 514–535.Google Scholar
  107. Woodward, A.L., and J.A. Sommerville. 2000. Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context. Psychological Science 11: 73–77.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of MemphisMemphisUSA
  2. 2.School of HumanitiesUniversity of HertfordshireHatfieldUK

Personalised recommendations