Advertisement

Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 2, Issue 2, pp 355–373 | Cite as

Shared Emotions and Joint Action

  • John MichaelEmail author
Joint Action: What is Shared?

Abstract

In recent years, several minimalist accounts of joint action have been offered (e.g. Tollefsen Philosophy of the Social Sciences 35:75–97, 2005; Sebanz et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6): 234–1246, 2006; Vesper et al. Neural Networks 23 (8/9): 998–1003, 2010), which seek to address some of the shortcomings of classical accounts. Minimalist accounts seek to reduce the cognitive complexity demanded by classical accounts either by leaving out shared intentions or by characterizing them in a way that does not demand common knowledge of complex, interconnected structures of intentions. Moreover, they propose models of the actual factors facilitating online coordination of movements. The present proposal aims to enrich a minimalist framework by showing how shared emotions can facilitate coordination without presupposing common knowledge of complex, interconnected structures of intentions. Shared emotions are defined for the purposes of this paper as affective states that fulfill two minimal criteria: (a) they are expressed (verbally or otherwise) by one person; and (b) the expression is perceived (consciously or unconsciously) by another person. Various ways in which the fulfillment of (a) and (b) can lead to effects that function as coordinating factors in joint action are distinguished and discussed.

Keywords

Joint Action Affective State Emotional Expression Joint Attention Shared Intention 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank everyone involved with the workshop “Shared emotions, joint attention, joint action” (Aarhus, 26th October 2010), without which this paper would not have been written—especially Anika Fiebich, Albert Newen, Andreas Roepstorff and Lars-Henrik Schmidt for making that event possible. I would also like to thank Steve Butterfill and one anonymous reviewer for very helpful comments, which greatly improved the paper.

References

  1. Apperly, I., and S. Butterfill. 2009. Do humans have two systems for tracking beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review 116(4): 953–970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Atmaca, S., N. Sebanz, W. Prinz, and G. Knoblich. 2008. Action co-representation: the joint SNARC effect. Social Neuroscience 3(3–4): 410–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Back, K.W. 1951. Influence through social communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 46: 9–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barsade, S. 2002. The ripple effect: emotional contagion and its effects on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly 47.Google Scholar
  5. Bernieri, F. 1988. Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 12(2): 120–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bower, G. 1981. Mood and Memory American Psychologist 36: 129–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bratman, M.E. 1992. Shared cooperative activity. The Philosophical Review 101(2): 327–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bratman, M.E. 1993. Shared intention. Ethics 104: 97–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bratman, M.E. 1997. I Intend that we J. In Contemporary action theory, volume 2: social action, ed. R. Tuomela and G. Holmstrom-Hintikka, 49–63. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  10. Bratman, M.E. 2009. Modest sociality and the distinctiveness of intention. Philosophical Studies 114(1): 149–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brownell, C.A., G.B. Ramani, and S. Zerwas. 2006. Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one-and two-year-olds. Child Development 77: 803–821.Google Scholar
  12. Charland, L. 1997. Reconciling cognitive and perceptual theories of emotion: a representational proposal. Philosophy of Science 64: 555–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chartrand, T., and J. Bargh. 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(6): 893–910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Damasio, A. 1994. Descartes’ error: emotions, reason and the human brain. London: Papermac.Google Scholar
  15. De Bruijn, E.R.A., F.P. deLange, D.Y. von Cramon, and M. Ullsperger. 2009. When errors are rewarding. The Journal of Neuroscience 29(39): 12183–12186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. De Bruijn, E.R.A., S.F. Miedl, and H. Bekkering. 2008. Fast responders have blinders on: ERP correlates of response inhibition in competition. Cortex 44: 580–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De Gelder, B. 2009. Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364: 3475–3484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. De Gelder, B. 2006. Toward the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 7: 242–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. De Sousa, R. “Emotion”, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2010 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URL = <http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2010/entries/emotion/>.
  20. De Sousa, R. 1987. The rationality of emotions. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  21. De Vignemont, F., and T. Singer. 2006. The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(10): 435–441.Google Scholar
  22. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., and Sadovsky, A. 2006. Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517–549).Google Scholar
  23. Ekman, P., and W.V. Friesen. 1989. The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In Handbook of social psychophysiology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Evans, D. 2002. The search hypothesis of emotion. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 53: 497–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Forgas, J., and J. George. 2001. Affective Influences on Judgments and Behavior in Organizations: An Information Processing Perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 86(1): 3–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Frijda, N. 1994. The Nature of Emotions: fundamental questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Gilbert, M. 1990. Walking together: a paradigmatic social phenomenon. Midwest Studies 15: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hatfield, E., and C. Hatfield. 1994. Emotional Contagion (Studies in Emotion & Social Interaction) Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press.Google Scholar
  29. Karau, S.J., and K.D. Williams. 1997. The effects of group cohesiveness on social loafing and social compensation. Group Dynamics 1: 156–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kelly, J., and S. Barsade. 2001. Mood contagion in small groups and work teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 86(1): 99–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lakin, J., and T. Chartrand. 2003. Using nonconcsious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science 14(4): 334–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lakin, J., Jefferis, V., Cheng, C.M., and T. Chartrand. 2003. The chameleon effect as social glue: the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27(3): 145–162.Google Scholar
  33. Liszkowski, U., M. Carpenter, A. Henning, T. Striano, and M. Tomasello. 2004. Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7: 297–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Locke, K., and J. Horowitz. 1990. Satisfaction in interpersonal interactions as a function of similarity in level of dysphoria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58(5): 823–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Loewenstein, G., and J. Lerner. 2003. The role of affect in decision making. In Handbook of Affective Science, ed. R. Davidson, H. Goldsmith, and K. Scherer, 619–642. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Michalak, J., N. Troje, J. Fischer, P. Vollmar, T. Heidenreich, and D. Schulte. 2009. Embodiment of sadness and depression—gait patterns associated with dysphoric mood. Psychosomatic Medicine 71: 580–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mullen, B., and C. Copper. 1994. The relation between group cohesiveness and performance: an integration. Psychological Bulletin 115: 210–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Newen, A., and A. Zinck. 2008. Classifying emotion: a developmental account. Synthese 161(1): 1–25.Google Scholar
  39. Öman, A., A. Flykt, and F. Esteves. 2001. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology 130(3): 466–478.Google Scholar
  40. Onishi, K.H., and R. Baillargeon. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308(8): 255–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pacherie, E., and J. Dokic. 2006. From mirror neurons to joint action. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 101–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Peacocke, C. 2005. Joint Attention: its nature, reflectivity, and connection to common knowledge. In Joint Attention, ed. J. Heal, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, and J. Roessler, 298–324. Communication and Other Minds London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Prinz, J. 2004. Gut reactions: a perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Repacholi, B.M., and A. Gopnik. 1997. Early reasoning about desires: evidence from14-and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology 33(1): 12–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rosati, C.S. (2008), Moral Motivation, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2008 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URL = <http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2008/entries/moral-motivation/>
  46. Sagi, A., and M.L. Hoffman. 1976. Empathic distress in newborns. Developmental Psychology 12: 175–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schachter, S. 1951. Deviation, rejection, and communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 46: 190–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Scherer, K. 2000. Psychological models of emotion. In Joan C. Borod (Ed.), The neuro-psychology of emotion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Searle, J.R. 1990. Collective intentions and actions. In Intentions in communication, ed. P. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. Pollack. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  50. Sebanz, N., G. Knoblich, and W. Prinz. 2005. Howtwoshareatask: corepresenting stimulus–response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 31(6): 1234–1246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Singer, T., and Lamm, C. 2009. The social neurosciecne of empathy, The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156: 81–96Google Scholar
  52. Sonnby-Borgström, M. 2002. Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 43(5): 433–443.Google Scholar
  53. Stern, D. 1985/1998. The interpersonal world of the infant: a view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  54. Surian, L., S. Caldi, and D. Sperber. 2007. Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science 18(7): 580–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tamietto, M., and B. de Gelder. 2008. Affective blindsight in the intact brain: neural inter- hemispheric summation for unseen fearful expressions. Neuropsychologia 46: 820–828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tollefsen, D. 2005. Let’s pretend! Joint action and young children. Philosophy of the Social Sciences 35: 75–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (5)Google Scholar
  58. Trevarthen, C. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech, ed. M. Bullowa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Troje, N. 2002. The little difference: fourier-based synthesis of gender-specific biological motion. In Dynamic perception, ed. R. Würtz and M. Lappe, 115–120. Berlin: AKA.Google Scholar
  60. Tuomela, R. 2005. WE-intentions revisited. Philosophical Studies 125: 327–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tsai, C.-C., W.-J. Kuo, D.L. Hung, and O.J.L. Tzeng. 2008. Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(11): 2015–2024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vaish, A., and T. Striano. 2004. Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science 7: 261–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. van Schie, H.T., R.B. Mars, M.G.H. Coles, and H. Bekkering. 2004. Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation. Nature Neuroscience 7(5): 549–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Vesper, C., S. Butterfill, N. Sebanz, and G. Knoblich. 2010. A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks 23(8/9): 998–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wells, A., and G. Matthews. 1994. Attention and emotion. A clinical perspective. East Sussex: Psychology.Google Scholar
  66. Wright, W., and G. Bower. 1992. Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 52(2): 276–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Post-Doctoral Research Fellow, GNOSIS Research CentreAarhus UniversityCopenhagenDenmark

Personalised recommendations