Advertisement

Mindfulness

pp 1–14 | Cite as

Time and Meditation: When Does the Perception of Time Change with Mindfulness Exercise?

  • Sylvie Droit-Volet
  • Magali Chaulet
  • Michaël Dambrun
ORIGINAL PAPER

Abstract

This study examined the prospective judgment of interval durations during a mindfulness meditation exercise in comparison with two control exercises involving different degrees of attentional demands and participants who either had or had not been trained to practice mindfulness exercises. The results showed that the interval durations (going from 15 to 60 s) were systematically judged shorter with the different mindfulness exercises (breathing, body scan) than with the control exercises. This underestimation of time was accompanied by the awareness that time seems to pass faster and by a decrease in the level of anxiety. However, the subjective feeling of the passage of time and the anxiety level did not explain time perception during a mindfulness meditation exercise. Further results suggest the critical role of attention in the effects of meditation on time judgments, a finding that is consistent with the idea that time flies during meditation as if time no longer existed.

Keywords

Mindfulness Time Experience of the passage of time Anxiety 

Notes

Author Contributions

SD: Designed the study, analyzed the data, wrote the manuscript, and edited the final manuscript. MD: Collaborated with the design of the study and writing of the study. MC: Recruited participants, executed the study, and assisted with the data analyses.

Funding Information

This study was funded by Horizon 2020 European Commission (H2020-FETPROACT-2014).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Statement

All procedures described in this manuscript were in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and were approved by the Sud-Est VI Statutory Ethics committee (CPP) in accordance with the French law on ethical experimentation.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all the individual participants included in study 1 and 2.

References

  1. Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Pratice, 1(2), 125–143.Google Scholar
  2. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., Walsh, E., Duggan, D., & Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15, 329–342.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bergoni, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2013). Measuring mindfulness: first step towards the development of a comprehensive mindfulness scale. Mindfulness, 4, 18–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2012). Mindfulness-induced changes in gamma band activity: implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(4), 700–710.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Block, R. A. (1992). Prospective and retrospective duration judgment: the role of information processing and memory. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition: towards bridging the gap (pp. 141–152). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1996). Models of psychological time revisited. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and Mind (pp. 171–195). Kirkand, WA: Hogrefe & Huver.Google Scholar
  7. Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: a meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 184–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-anaytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 330–333.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Droit-Volet, S., & Heros, J. (2017). Time judgments as a function of mindfulness meditation, anxiety and mindfulness awareness. Mindfulness, 8(2), 266–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Droit-Volet, S., & Wearden, J. (2016). Passage of time judgments are not duration judgments: evidence from a study using experience sampling methodology. Frontiers in Psychology, Section Cognition, 7, 176.Google Scholar
  11. Droit-Volet, S., Fayolle, S., Lamotte, M., & Gil, S. (2013). Time, emotion and the embodiment of timing. Timing and Time Perception, 1(1), 99–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Droit-Volet, S., Fanget, M., & Dambrun, M. (2015). Mindfulness meditation and relaxation training increases time sensitivity. Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 85–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Droit-Volet, S., Trahanias, P., & Maniadakis, M. (2017). Passage of time judgments in everyday life are not related to duration judgments except for long durations of several minutes. Acta Psychologica, 73, 116–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Droit-Volet, S., Monceau, S., Berthon, M., Trahanias, P., & Maniadakis, M. (submitted). The explicit judgment of long durations of several minutes in everyday life: conscious retrospective memory judgment and the role of affects?Google Scholar
  15. Eysenck, M. W., & Byrne, A. (1992). Anxiety and susceptibility to distraction. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 793–798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 955–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gibbon, J., Church, R., & Meck, W. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423: timing and time perception (pp. 52–77). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  20. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1(2), 144–156.Google Scholar
  21. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses. New York: Hyperion.Google Scholar
  22. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2016). This is not McMindfulness. The Psychologist, 29(2), 124–125.Google Scholar
  23. Kramer, R. S. S., Weger, U. W., & Sharma, D. (2013). The effect of mindfulness on time perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 846–852.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Lake, J. I., & Meck, W. H. (2013). Differential effects of amphetamine and haloperidol on temporal reproduction: dopaminergic regulation of attention and clock speed. Neuropsychologia, 51, 284–292.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Maricq, A. V., & Church, R. M. (1983). The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 79, 10–15.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 301–306.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 102–111.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavioral Processes, 9, 171–201.Google Scholar
  31. Nobre, A. C., & Coull, J. T. (2010). Attention and time. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Otten, S., Schötz, E., Wittmann, M., Kohls, N., Schmidt, S., & Meissner, K. (2015). Psychophysiology of duration estimation in experienced mindfulness mediators and matched controls. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.Google Scholar
  33. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Method, Instruments and Computer, 36, 717–731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Prévert, J. (1949). Paroles. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  35. Schötz, E., Otten, S., Wittmann, M., Schimidt, S., Kohls, N., & Meissner, K. (2016). Time perception, mindfulness and attentional capacities in transcendental meditators and matched controls. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 16–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6), 1139–1171.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  38. Sucala, M., & David, D. (2013). Mindful about time in a fast forward world. The effects of mindfulness exercise on time perception. Transylvanian Journal of Psychology, 14, 243–253.Google Scholar
  39. Tang, Y.-Y., Hôlzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews, Neurosciences, 16, 213–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Thönes, S., & Wittmann, M. (2016). Time perception in yogic mindfulness meditation: effects on retrospective duration judgments and time passage. Psychology of Consiousness: Theory, Research and Pratice, 3(4), 316–325.Google Scholar
  41. Walach, H., Buchhled, N., Buttenmüller, V., Kleinknecht, N., & Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI). Personnality Individual Diffferences, 40, 1543–1555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Weiner, L., Wittmann, M., Bertschy, G., & Giersch, A. (2016). Dispositional mindfulness and subjective time in healthy individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 786.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Wittmann, M., Peter, J., Gutina, O., Otten, S., Kohls, N., & Meissner, K. (2014). Individual differences in self-attributed mindfulness levels are related to the experience of time and cognitive self-control. Personality and Individual Difference, 64, 41–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wittmann, M., Otten, S., Schötz, E., Sarikaya, A., Lehnen, H., Jo, H.-G., Kohls, N., Schmidt, S., & Meissnrr, K. (2015). Subjective expansion of extend time-spans in experienced meditators. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1586), 1–9.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sylvie Droit-Volet
    • 1
  • Magali Chaulet
    • 1
  • Michaël Dambrun
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et Cognitive (LAPSCO), CNRS, UMR 6024Université Clermont AuvergneClermont-FerrandFrance

Personalised recommendations