Mindfulness

, Volume 8, Issue 1, pp 17–26 | Cite as

Concentrative Meditation Influences Visual Awareness: a Study with Color Afterimages

ORIGINAL PAPER

Abstract

While many studies have shown that meditation enhances attentional processing, very few studies have investigated the effect of enhanced attentional processing on visual awareness. We investigate the attentional effects on visual awareness in focused attention meditators using a task that manipulates scope of attention using hierarchical letter stimuli (local and global processing) and single letter stimuli of varying size (small and large). In addition, working memory load was manipulated using a 0-back and 2-back task. Data were collected from Sahaj Samadhi Meditators and an age-matched control group of non-meditators. Visual awareness was tapped using negative color afterimages by measuring the duration and more importantly the clarity and color of afterimages using a rating scale. The afterimage durations were significantly longer for Sahaj Samadhi meditators compared to non-meditators. In addition, the afterimages were sharper for meditators compared to non-meditators suggesting that better attentional focusing associated with meditators might lead to phenomenal changes in visual awareness. Scope of attention influenced not only afterimage durations but also clarity indicating that changes in scope also influence aspects of visual awareness. The results indicate meditation training not only modifies attentional processes but also results in changes in conscious visual perception.

Keywords

Concentrative meditation Awareness Attention Working memory Afterimages Consciousness 

References

  1. Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience and working memory interact. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 166–172.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Types of attention matter for awareness: a study with color afterimages. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 1039–1048.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baijal, S., & Srinivasan, N. (2011). Consolidation of statistical information of multiple objects in working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1733–1741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 11483–11488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Buttle, H. (2011). Attention and working memory in mindfulness. The Journal of mind and behaviour, 32(2), 123–134.Google Scholar
  6. Carrasco, M. (2009). Cross-modal attention enhances perceived contrast. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(52), 22039–22040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carrasco, M. (2014). Spatial attention: Perceptual modulation. In S. Kastner & A. C. Nobre (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Attention (pp. 183–230). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 308–313.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Carte, O. L., Prest, D. E., Callistemon, C., Ungerer, Y., Liu, G. B., & Pettigrew, J. D. (2005). Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Tibetan Buddhist monks. Current Biology, 15(11), R412–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chambers, R., Lo, B. C., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chan, D., & Woollcott, M. (2007). Effects of levels of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13(6), 651–657.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Chaudhuri, A. (1990). Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention. Nature, 344, 60–62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chong, S. C., & Treisman, A. (2005). Attentional spread in the statistical processing of visual displays. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 1–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. New York and Hove: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(5), 204–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Demeyere, N., Rzeskiewicz, A., Humphreys, K. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Automatic statistical processing of visual properties in simultanagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2861–2864.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Friese, M., Messner, C., & Schaffner, Y. (2012). Mindfulness meditation counteracts self-control depletion. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1016–1022.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Fuller, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Exogenous attention and color perception: Performance and appearance of saturation and hue. Vision Research, 46, 4032–4047.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10(1), 83–91.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Hodgins, H. S., & Adairs, K. C. (2010). Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 872–878.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 109–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jha, A., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., & Wong, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54–64.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kemeny, M. E., Foltz, C., Cavanagh, J. F., Cullen, M., Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P., & Ekman, P. (2012). Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative emotional behavior. Emotion, 12, 338–350.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Kirschfield, K. (1999). Afterimages: a tool for defining the neural correlate of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 462–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 16–22.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Lamme, V. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Lavie, N. (2006). The role of perceptual load on visual awareness. Brain Research, 1080, 91–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Lou, L. (1999). Selective peripheral fading: Evidence for inhibitory sensory effect of attention. Perception, 28, 519–526.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Lou, L. (2001). Effects of voluntary attention on structured afterimages. Perception, 30, 1439–1448.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. Plos One, e1897.Google Scholar
  31. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., & Greischar, L. L. (2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(42), 13418–13427.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (2001). Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  33. MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., & Saron, C. D. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001). Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature, 4, 739–744.Google Scholar
  35. Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: the mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91(16), 7938–7403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Prinzmetal, W., Nwachuku, I., Bodanski, L., Blumenfeld, L., & Shizimu, N. (1997). The phenomenology of attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 372–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Prinzmetal, W., Amiri, H., Allen, K., & Edwards, T. (1998). The phenomenology of attention Part 1: Color, location, orientation and spatial frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Performance and Perception, 24, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cognitive Processing, 11, 1–7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 245–277.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Robins, C. J., Keng, S., Ekblad, A. G., & Brantley, J. G. (2012). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on emotional experience and expression: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(1), 117–131.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Rose, D., Bradshaw, M. F., & Hibbard, P. B. (2003). Attention affects the stereoscopic depth aftereffect. Perception, 32, 635–640.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., et al. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLOS Biology, e138.Google Scholar
  43. Srinivasan, N., & Baijal, S. (2007). Concentrative meditation enhances pre-attentive processing: a mismatch negativity study. Neuroreport, 18, 1709–1712.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Suzuki, S., & Grabowecky, M. (2003). Attention during adaptation weakens negative afterimages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 793–807.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short term meditation training improves attention and self regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17152–17156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Triesman, A. (2006). How deployment of attention determines what we see. Visual Cognition, 14, 411–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101.Google Scholar
  48. Vugt, M., & Jha, A. (2011). Investigating the impact of mindfulness meditation training on working memory: a mathematical modeling approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 344–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wede, J., & Francis, G. (2007). Attentional effects on afterimages: Theory and data. Vision Research, 47, 2249–2258.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Zeidana, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre of Behavioural and Cognitive SciencesUniversity of AllahabadAllahabadIndia

Personalised recommendations