Psychological Studies

, Volume 60, Issue 3, pp 249–256 | Cite as

Differential Effects of Irrelevant Emotional Context on Regret and Rejoice: A Behavioural Economic Investigation of Decision Making under Risk

  • Debarati Bandyopadhyay
  • Narayanan Srinivasan
  • V. S. Chandrasekhar Pammi
Research in Progress

Abstract

This study investigates the role of incidental emotion and probability on two aspects of decision making: decision time and post-choice satisfaction (regret and rejoice). We used a modified regret paradigm in which people made choices in the context of pleasant, neutral and unpleasant emotional International Affective Picture System (IAPS) pictures present throughout the decision making and post-decision stages. Both pleasant and unpleasant emotional contexts led to a slower decision time compared to neutral context. With respect to post-choice feeling, participants experienced more regret after losing and less rejoice after winning in unpleasant compared to pleasant and neutral contexts. Further, we observed decreased regret rating from low to high risk conditions in the pleasant context. The regret rating was also observed to be more in the unpleasant compared to the neutral and pleasant contexts during low and high risk conditions, respectively. The results suggest that incidental emotions present during decision making do influence both decision time and post-choice ratings. These results imply that decision making theories need to consider incidental emotional context to explain decision making. We also discuss similarities and differences when incidental emotions are present during decision making with incidental emotions that are present prior to decision making.

Keywords

Decision making Risk Emotions Win Loss Regret Rejoice 

References

  1. Arkes, H. R., Herren, L. T., & Isen, A. M. (1988). The role of potential loss in the influence of affect on risk-taking behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 42, 181–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandyopadhyay, D., Pammi, V. S. C., & Srinivasan, N. (2013a). Role of affect in decision making. Progress in Brain Research, 202, 37–53.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bandyopadhyay, D., Srinivasan, N., & Pammi, V. S. C. (2013b). Dissociable effects of task irrelevant emotional information on decision making under risk. Neuroscience of Decision Making, 1, 1–8. doi:10.2478/ndm-2013-0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–12.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215–225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bell, D. E. (1982). Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 30, 961–981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bless, H. (2000). The interplay of affect and cognition: the mediating role of general knowledge structures. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: the role of affect in social cognition (pp. 201–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias: the case of anchoring. Psychological Science, 11, 320–323.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Capra, C. M. (2004). Mood-driven behavior in strategic interactions. American Economic Review, 94, 367–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chandrasekhar, P. V., Capra, C. M., Moore, S., Noussair, C., & Berns, G. S. (2008). Neurobiological regret and rejoice functions for aversive outcomes. NeuroImage, 39, 1472–1484.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Towards understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In A. H. Hastorf & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive Social Psychology (pp. 73–108). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  14. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. NY: Grosset/Putnam.Google Scholar
  15. Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Fiedler & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 25–43). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  16. Engelmann, J. B., & Tamir, D. (2009). Individual differences in risk preference predict neural responses during financial decision-making. Brain Research, 1290, 28–51.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fehr-Duda, H., Epper, T., Bruhin, A., & Schubert, R. (2011). Risk and rationality: the effects of mood and decision rules on probability weighting. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 78, 14–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fielder, K. (2000). Towards an integrative account of affect and cognition phenomena using the BIAS computer algorithm. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: the role of affect in social cognition (pp. 223–252). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Forgas, J. P. (1998). Happy and mistaken? Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 318–331.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Forgas, J. P. (2003). Affective influences on attitudes and judgments. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer and H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences, (596–618). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Gasper, K., & Clore, G. (2002). Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., Mangun, G. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2002). Emotion. In M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, & G. R. Mangun (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind (pp. 537–576). New York: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
  24. Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer decision making: the appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17, 158–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Harlé, K. M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the Ultimatum Game. Emotion, 7, 876–881.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Isen, A. M. (1984). Towards understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (3rd ed., pp. 179–236). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 521–539). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Isen, A. M., & Geva, N. (1987). The influence of positive affect on acceptable level of risk: the person with a large canoe has a large worry. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 39, 145–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Isen, A. M., Nygren, T. E., & Ashby, F. G. (1988). Influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: it is just not worth the risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(5), 710–717.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse strings: carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science, 15, 337–341.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. J. Dawson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 619–642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Loewenstein, G., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–86.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Lohani, M., Gupta, R., & Srinivasan, N. (in press, 2013). Cross-cultural evaluation of the international affective picture system on an Indian sample. Psychological Studies.Google Scholar
  36. Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: an alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. Economic Journal, 92, 805–824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. (1989). Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 910–924.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8, 423–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Olivers, C. N., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2006). The beneficial effects of additional task load, positive affect, and instruction on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 364–79.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and Heuristics: the influence of happy and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(6), 1005–1023.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (2nd ed., pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  44. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution and judgment of well-being. Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Steffen, A. C., Rockstroh, B., & Jansma, B. M. (2009). Brain evoked potentials reflect how emotional faces influence our decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2, 32–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973–988.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., Manstead, A. S. R., & van der Pligt, J. (2000). On bad decisions and disconfirmed expectancies: the psychology of regret and disappointment. Emotion and Cognition, 14, 521–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© National Academy of Psychology (NAOP) India 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Debarati Bandyopadhyay
    • 1
  • Narayanan Srinivasan
    • 1
  • V. S. Chandrasekhar Pammi
    • 1
  1. 1.Centre of Behavioural and Cognitive SciencesUniversity of AllahabadAllahabadIndia

Personalised recommendations