Advertisement

Cognitive Computation

, Volume 8, Issue 1, pp 1–14 | Cite as

Emotion: A Unified Mechanistic Interpretation from a Cognitive Architecture

Article

Abstract

This paper reviews a project that attempts to interpret emotion, a complex and multifaceted phenomenon, from a mechanistic point of view, facilitated by an existing comprehensive computational cognitive architecture—CLARION. This cognitive architecture consists of a number of subsystems: the action-centered, non-action-centered, motivational, and metacognitive subsystems. From this perspective, emotion is, first and foremost, motivationally based. It is also action-oriented. It involves many other identifiable cognitive functionalities within these subsystems. Based on these functionalities, we fit the pieces together mechanistically (computationally) within the CLARION framework and capture a variety of important aspects of emotion as documented in the literature.

Keywords

Emotion Cognitive architecture Psychology Computational 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work has been supported in part by the ONR Grants N00014-08-1-0068 and N00014-13-1-0342.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Ron Sun, Nick Wilson, and Michael Lynch declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008 (5). Additional informed consent was obtained from all patients for which identifying information is included in this article.

Human and Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. 1.
    Bach J. Principles of synthetic intelligence PSI: an architecture of motivated cognition. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Bower GH. Mood and memory. Am Psychol. 1981;36:129–48.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Carver C, Scheier M. On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Clark LA, Watson D. Temperament: a new paradigm for trait psychology. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality: theory and research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. p. 399–423.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Damasio A. Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Penguin; 1994.Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Dorner D. The mathematics of emotion. In: Proceedings of the fifth international conference on cognitive modeling. Germany: Bamberg; 2003. p. 75–79.Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Ekman P. Basic emotions. In: Dalgleish T, Power M, editors. Handbook of cognition and emotion. Sussex: Wiley; 1999.Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Evans J, Frankish K, editors. In two minds: dual processes and beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009.Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Flavell J. Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In: Resnick B, editor. The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1976.Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Frijda N. The emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986.Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Helie S, Sun R. Incubation, insight, and creative problem solving: a unified theory and a connectionist model. Psychol Rev. 2010;117(3):994–1024.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Higgins ET. Beyond pleasure and pain. Am Psychol. 1997;52(12):1280–300.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Hudlicka E. A computational model of emotion and personality: applications to psychotherapy research and practice. In: Proceedings of the 10th annual cybertherapy conference: a decade of virtual reality. Switzerland: Basel; 2005.Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Hull C. Essentials of behavior. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1951.Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Lambie J, Marcel A. Consciousness and the variety of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychol Rev. 2002;109(2):219–59.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Lazarus RS. Cognition and motivation in emotion. Am Psychol. 1991;46(4):352–67.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Lazarus RS. Relational meaning and discrete emotions. In: Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal processes in emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001.Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer; 1984.Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Ledoux J. Brain mechanisms of emotion and emotional learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1992;2(2):191–7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Leven S, Levine D. Multiattribute decision making in context: a dynamic neural network methodology. Cogn Sci. 1996;20:271–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Lewis M. Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behav Brain Sci. 2004.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Marinier RP III, Laird JE, Lewis RL. A computational unification of cognitive behavior and emotion. Cogn Syst Res. 2009;10(1):48–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Marsella S, Gratch J. EMA: a process model of appraisal dynamics. Cogn Syst Res. 2009;10(1):70–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Maslow A. Motivation and personality. 3rd ed. New York: Harper and Row; 1987.Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Miceli M, Castelfranchi C. Expectancy and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2014.Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Mineka S, Sutton S. Cognitive biases and the emotional disorders. Psychol Sci. 1992;3(1):65–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Mueller ET, Dyer MG. Daydreaming in humans and computers. In: Proceedings of the ninth international joint conference on artificial intelligence. Los Angeles: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers, Inc.; 1985. p. 18–24.Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Murphy ST, Zajonc RB. Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:723–39.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Murray H. Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press; 1938.Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Ortony A, Clore G, Collins A. The cognitive structures of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Reber A. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1989;118(3):219–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Reder L, editor. Implicit memory and metacognition. Mahwah: Erlbaum; 1996.Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Reisenzein R. Emotions as metarepresentational states of mind: naturalizing the belief-desire theory of emotion. Cogn Syst Res. 2009;10(1):6–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Reiss S. Multifaceted nature of intrinsic motivation: the theory of 16 basic desires. Rev Gen Psychol. 2004;8(3):179–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Rumelhart D, McClelland J, PDP Research Group. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructures of cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1986.Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Schacter D. Implicit memory: history and current status. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1987;13:501–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Scherer K. Studying the emotion-antecedent appraisal process: the expert system approach. Cogn Emot. 1993;7:325–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Scherer KR. Emotion and emotional competence: conceptual and theoretical issues for modelling agents. In: Scherer K, Banziger T, Roesch E, editors. A blueprint for affective computing. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010. p. 3–20.Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal processes in emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001.Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    Simon HA. Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychol Rev. 1967;74:29–39.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Smillie LD, Pickering AD, Jackson CJ. The new reinforcement sensitivity theory: implications for personality measurement. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006;10:320–35.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Smith CA, Lazarus RS. Emotion and adaptation. In: Pervin LA, editor. Handbook on personality: theory and research. New York: Guilford; 1990. p. 609–37.Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Sun R. Duality of the mind. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002.Google Scholar
  45. 45.
    Sun R. A tutorial on CLARION 5.0. Technical report, Cognitive Sciences Department, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, Troy. 2003. http://www.cogsci.rpi.edu/~rsun/sun.tutorial.pdf.
  46. 46.
    Sun R. Motivational representations within a computational cognitive architecture. Cogn Comput. 2009;1(1):91–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. 47.
    Sun R. Theoretical status of computational cognitive modeling. Cogn Syst Res. 2009;10(2):124–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. 48.
    Sun R. Memory systems within a cognitive architecture. New Ideas Psychol. 2012;30:227–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Sun R. Anatomy of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016.Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Sun R, Fleischer P. A cognitive social simulation of tribal survival strategies: the importance of cognitive and motivational factors. J Cogn Cult. 2012;12(3–4):287–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Sun, R, Mathews R. Implicit cognition, emotion, and meta-cognitive control. Mind Soc. Special issue on Dual Processes Theories of Thinking (edited by D. Over, L. Macchi, and R. Viale), 2012;11(1):107–119.Google Scholar
  52. 52.
    Sun R, Wilson N. A model of personality should be a cognitive architecture itself. Cogn Syst Res. 2014;29–30:1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. 53.
    Sun R, Zhang X. Accounting for a variety of reasoning data within a cognitive architecture. J Exp Theor Artif Intell. 2006;18(2):169–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Sun R, Merrill E, Peterson T. From implicit skills to explicit knowledge: a bottom-up model of skill learning. Cogn Sci. 2001;25(2):203–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    Sun R, Slusarz P, Terry C. The interaction of the explicit and the implicit in skill learning: a dual-process approach. Psychol Rev. 2005;112(1):159–92.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    Toates F. Motivational systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986.Google Scholar
  57. 57.
    Tolman EC. Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century; 1932.Google Scholar
  58. 58.
    Tyrell T. Computational mechanisms for action selection. Ph.D. Thesis, Oxford: Oxford University; 1993.Google Scholar
  59. 59.
    Watkins C. Learning with delayed rewards. Ph.D. Thesis, Cambridge: Cambridge University; 1989.Google Scholar
  60. 60.
    Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063–79.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Wilson NR. Towards a psychologically plausible comprehensive computational theory of emotion. Ph.D Thesis, Cognitive Sciences Department, Troy: Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute; (2012).Google Scholar
  62. 62.
    Wilson N, Sun R. Coping with bullying: a computational emotion-theoretic account. In: Proceedings of the annual conference of cognitive science society, Quebec City, Quebec, Canada. p. 3119–24. Published by Cognitive Science Society, Austin, Texas (2014).Google Scholar
  63. 63.
    Wilson N, Sun R, Mathews R. A motivationally-based simulation of performance degradation under pressure. Neural Netw. 2009;22:502–8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. 64.
    Wilson N, Sun R, Mathews R. A motivationally based computational interpretation of social anxiety induced stereotype bias. In: Proceedings of the annual conference of cognitive science society, Oregon: Portland. p. 1750–55; 2010.Google Scholar
  65. 65.
    Winkielman P, Berridge K, Wilbarger J. Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005;31(1):121–35.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Wright IP, Sloman A. MINDER1: an implementation of a proto-emotional agent architecture. Technical Report CSRP-97-1, University of Birmingham, School of Computer Science; 1997.Google Scholar
  67. 67.
    Zajonc RB. Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inference. Am Psychol. 1980;35(2):151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Cognitive Sciences DepartmentRensselaer Polytechnic InstituteTroyUSA

Personalised recommendations