Advertisement

International Journal of Social Robotics

, Volume 10, Issue 5, pp 687–700 | Cite as

Age- and Gender-Based Differences in Children’s Interactions with a Gender-Matching Robot

  • Anara SandygulovaEmail author
  • Gregory M. P. O’Hare
Article

Abstract

Social robots are increasingly being used to encourage social, emotional and cognitive growth in children. However, in order to establish social and bonding interactions, social robots need to be able to exhibit adaptive strategies to keep children engaged and interested. Adaptive strategies of a social robot based on children’s age and gender are motivated by the comprehensive theory on gender development. Given the strong influence of gender in children’s cognitive development, the experiment first examined the responses of 107 children, ages 5–12, whether synthesized voice evokes gender associations in children. The results suggest that young children (ages 5–8) are not able to successfully attribute gender to the robot in correspondence with the synthesized voice. In addition, we explicitly investigated children’s preferences for the robot’s gender, and the results were contrary to our expectations: young children indicated their preference for a robot with a matching gender while there was no difference in preferences for a robot’s gender by older children (ages 9–12).

Keywords

Child–robot interaction Human–robot interaction Social robotics Adaptive strategies Gender Age 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would also like to express great appreciation to the members of staff of the Queen of Angels primary school for their time and help with the experiment. Special thanks to Dr. Maria Nikolayev (Sumaroka), who provided very valuable advice and suggestions in designing the experiment. In addition, great gratitude should be given to Dr. Verena Nitsch for her help with data analysis. The authors would like to thank the reviewers for their valuable feedback and suggestions to improve the article.

Funding

This study was funded by Irish Research Council and Science Foundation Ireland (07/CE/l1147).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflicts of interest

Anara Sandygulova has received research grants from Irish Research Council.

References

  1. 1.
    Acapela group. http://www.acapela-group.com/. Accessed 30 Aug 2015
  2. 2.
    Alonso-Martín F, Malfaz M, Sequeira J, Gorostiza JF, Salichs MA (2013) A multimodal emotion detection system during human–robot interaction. Sensors 13(11):15549–15581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Auster CJ, Mansbach CS (2012) The gender marketing of toys: an analysis of color and type of toy on the disney store website. Sex Roles 67(7–8):375–388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Bell A (2007) Designing and testing questionnaires for children. J Res Nurs 12(5):461–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Belpaeme T, Baxter P, de Greeff J, Kennedy J, Read R, Looije R, Neerincx M, Baroni I, Zelati M (2013) Child-robot interaction: perspectives and challenges. In: Herrmann G, Pearson M, Lenz A, Bremner P, Spiers A, Leonards U (eds) Social robotics, volume 8239 of Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Springer, Berlin, pp 452–459Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Belpaeme T, Baxter P, Read R, Wood R, Cuayáhuitl H, Kiefer B, Racioppa S, Kruijff-Korbayová I, Athanasopoulos G, Enescu V, Looije R, Neerincx M, Demiris Y, Ros-Espinoza R, Beck A, Cañamero L, Hiolle A, Lewis M, Baroni I, Nalin M, Cosi P, Paci G, Tesser F, Sommavilla G, Humbert R (2012) Multimodal child-robot interaction: building social bonds. J Hum-Robot Interact 1(2):33–53Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Belpaeme T, Kennedy J, Baxter P, Vogt P, Krahmer EE, Kopp S, Bergmann K, Leseman P, Küntay AC, Göksun T et al (2015) L2TOR-second language tutoring using social robots. In: Proceedings of the ICSR 2015 WONDER workshopGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Chiasson S, Gutwin C (2005) Testing the media equation with children. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems, CHI ’05, New York, NY, USA. ACM, pp 829–838Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Clark C (2010) In a younger voice: doing child-centered qualitative research. Child development in cultural context series. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Cook J, Cook G (2003) Child development: principles and perspectives. Allyn & Bacon, BostonGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Crowelly C, Villanoy M, Scheutzz M, Schermerhornz P (2009) Gendered voice and robot entities: perceptions and reactions of male and female subjects. In: IEEE/RSJ international conference on intelligent robots and systems, 2009. IROS 2009. IEEE, pp 3735–3741Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Egan SK, Perry DG (2001) Gender identity: a multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Dev Psychol 37(4):451–463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Ernst A, Ruf T, Kueblbeck C (2009) A modular framework to detect and analyze faces for audience measurement systems. In: 2nd workshop on pervasive advertising at informatik, pp 75–87Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Eyssel F, Kuchenbrandt D, Bobinger S, de Ruiter L, Hegel F (2012) ‘If you sound like me, you must be more human’: on the interplay of robot and user features on human–robot acceptance and anthropomorphism. In: Proceedings of the seventh annual ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction. ACM, pp 125–126Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Fine C (2010) Delusions of gender: how our minds, society, and neurosexism create difference. WW Norton & Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Fink J, Lemaignan S, Dillenbourg P, Rétornaz P, Vaussard F, Berthoud A, Mondada F, Wille F, Franinović K (2014) Which robot behavior can motivate children to tidy up their toys? In: Proceedings of the 2014 ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction—HRI ’14, New York, NY, USA. ACM Press, pp 439–446Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Foley S, Linnehan F, Greenhaus JH, Weer CH (2006) The impact of gender similarity, racial similarity, and work culture on family-supportive supervision. Group Org Manag 31(4):420–441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Fussell SR, Kiesler S, Setlock LD, Yew V (2008) How people anthropomorphize robots. In: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on human robot interaction. ACM, pp 145–152Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Heyman GD (2001) Children’s interpretation of ambiguous behavior: evidence for a ‘boys are bad’ bias. Soc Dev 10(2):230–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Hood D, Lemaignan S, Dillenbourg P (2015) The CoWriter project: teaching a robot how to write. In: Proceedings of the tenth annual ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction extended abstracts. ACM, pp 269–269Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Huston AC (1983) Sex-typing. In: Mussen PH, Hetherington EM (eds) Handbook of child psychology, vol 4. Socialization, personality and social behavior, 4th edn. Wiley, New York, pp 387–467Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Johnson J (2003) Children, robotics, and education. Artif Life Robot 7(1–2):16–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Kanda T, Hirano T, Eaton D, Ishiguro H (2004) Interactive robots as social partners and peer tutors for children: a field trial. Hum Comput Interact 19(1):61–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Katibeh Z (2013) Techniques for real-time multi-person face tracking for human–robot dialogue. Ph.D. thesis, Blekinge Institute of TechnologyGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Kaufman SB, Singer JL, Singer DG (2012) The need for pretend play in child development. Psychol TodayGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Kennedy J, Baxter P, Belpaeme T (2015) The robot who tried too hard: social behaviour of a robot tutor can negatively affect child learning. In: Proceedings of the tenth annual ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction. ACM, pp 67–74Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Kose-Bagci H, Ferrari E, Dautenhahn K, Syrdal DS, Nehaniv CL (2009) Effects of embodiment and gestures on social interaction in drumming games with a humanoid robot. Adv Robot 23(14):1951–1996CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Kriz S, Anderson G, Trafton J, Bugajska M (2009) Robot-directed speech as a means of exploring conceptualizations of robots. In: 2009 4th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI), pp 271–272Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Lee KM, Liao K, Ryu S (2007) Children’s responses to computer-synthesized speech in educational media: gender consistency and gender similarity effects. Hum Commun Res 33(3):310–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Leite I, Castellano G, Pereira A, Martinho C, Paiva A (2012) Long-term interactions with empathic robots: evaluating perceived support in children. In: Ge SS, Khatib O, Cabibihan JJ, Simmons R, Williams M-A (eds) Social robotics. Springer, Berlin, pp 298–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Maccoby EE (1988) Gender as a social category. Dev Psychol 24(6):755CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Martin CL, Ruble DN (2010) Patterns of gender development. Annu Rev Psychol 61:353–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Mehta CM, Strough J (2009) Sex segregation in friendships and normative contexts across the life span. Dev Rev 29(3):201–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Meiirbekov S, Balkibekov K, Jalankuzov Z, Sandygulova A (2016) You win, I lose: towards adapting robot’s teaching strategy. In: The Eleventh ACM/IEEE international conference on human robot interaction. IEEE Press, pp 475–476Google Scholar
  35. 35.
    Nass CI, Brave S (2005) Wired for speech: how voice activates and advances the human–computer relationship. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Ozogul G, Johnson AM, Atkinson RK, Reisslein M (2013) Investigating the impact of pedagogical agent gender matching and learner choice on learning outcomes and perceptions. Comput Educ 67:36–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Powers A, Kramer AD, Lim S, Kuo J, Lee S-l, Kiesler S (2005) Eliciting information from people with a gendered humanoid robot. In: IEEE international workshop on robot and human interactive communication, 2005. ROMAN 2005. IEEE, pp 158–163Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Powlishta KK, Serbin LA, Doyle A-B, White DR (1994) Gender, ethnic, and body type biases: the generality of prejudice in childhood. Dev Psychol 30(4):526–536CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Rao AS, Georgeff MP et al (1995) BDI agents: from theory to practice. ICMAS 95:312–319Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Reeves B, Nass C (1996) The media equation: how people treat computers, television, and new media like real people and places. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Ros R, Nalin M, Wood R, Baxter P, Looije R, Demiris Y, Belpaeme T, Giusti A, Pozzi C (2011) Child–robot interaction in the wild: advice to the aspiring experimenter. In: Proceedings of the 13th international conference on multimodal interfaces, ICMI ’11, New York, NY, USA. ACM, pp 335–342Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Ruble DN, Martin CL, Berenbaum SA (2007) Gender development. Wiley, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Sandygulova A,  Dragone M, O’Hare GM (2014) Investigating the impact of gender development in child–robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the 3rd international symposium on new frontiers in human–robot interaction at AISB 2014, Goldsmiths College, London, UKGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Sandygulova A, O’Hare GM (2015) Children’s perception of synthesized voice: robot’s gender, age and accent. In: Tapus A, André E, Martin J-C, Ferland F, Ammi M (eds) Social robotics. Springer, Cham, pp 594–602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Sandygulova A, O’Hare GM (2016) Investigating the impact of gender segregation within observational pretend play interaction. In: 2016 11th ACM/IEEE international conference on human–robot interaction (HRI). IEEE, pp 399–406Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Scheeff M, Pinto J, Rahardja K, Snibbe S, Tow R (2002) Experiences with Sparky, a social robot. In: Socially intelligent agents, pp 173–180Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Schermerhorn P, Scheutz M, Crowell CR (2008) Robot social presence and gender: Do females view robots differently than males? In: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE international conference on human robot interaction. ACM, pp 263–270Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Serbin LA, Moller LC, Gulko J, Powlishta KK, Colburne KA (1994) The emergence of gender segregation in toddler playgroups. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev 65:7–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Siegel M, Breazeal C, Norton MI (2009) Persuasive robotics: the influence of robot gender on human behavior. In: IEEE/RSJ international conference on intelligent robots and systems, 2009. IROS 2009. IEEE, pp 2563–2568Google Scholar
  50. 50.
  51. 51.
    Tamagawa R, Watson C, Kuo I, MacDonald B, Broadbent E (2011) The effects of synthesized voice accents on user perceptions of robots. Int J Soc Robot 3(3):253–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Tung F-W (2011) Influence of gender and age on the attitudes of children towards humanoid robots. In: Jacko J (ed) Human–computer interaction. Users and applications, volume 6764 of Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Springer, Berlin, pp 637–646Google Scholar
  53. 53.
    Verkuyten M, Thijs J (2001) Ethnic and gender bias among Dutch and Turkish children in late childhood: the role of social context. Infant Child Dev 10(4):203–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Wang Y, Young JE (2014) Beyond “pink” and “blue”: gendered attitudes towards robots in society. In: Proceedings of gender and IT appropriation. Science and practice on dialogue—forum for interdisciplinary exchange, gender IT ’14, Siegen, Germany, European Society for Socially Embedded Technologies, pp 49:49–49:59Google Scholar
  55. 55.
    Woods S (2006) Exploring the design space of robots: children’s perspectives. Interact Comput 18(6):1390–1418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    Yee M, Brown R (1994) The development of gender differentiation in young children. Brit J Soc Psychol 33(2):183–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. 57.
    Zosuls KM, Miller CF, Ruble DN, Martin CL, Fabes RA (2011) Gender development research in sex roles: historical trends and future directions. Sex Roles 64(11–12):826–842CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Zora. http://zorarobotics.be/?lg=en/ Accessed 30 Aug 2015

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Science and TechnologyNazarbayev UniversityAstanaKazakhstan
  2. 2.University College DublinBelfield, Dublin 4Ireland

Personalised recommendations