Biosemiotics

pp 1–23 | Cite as

Umwelt and Ape Language Experiments: on the Role of Iconicity in the Human-Ape Pidgin Language

Article
  • 33 Downloads

Abstract

Several language experiments have been carried out on apes and other animals aiming to narrow down the presumed qualitative gap that separates humans from other animals. These experiments, however, have been driven by the understanding of language as a purely symbolic sign system, often connected to a profound disinterest for language use in real situations and a propensity to perceive grammatical and syntactic information as the only fundamental aspects of human language. For these reasons, the language taught to apes tends to discard iconic and indexical elements in favour of symbolic signs. This paper sheds light on the iconic components of human language, with close attention to the iconic properties of language as present in the ape language experiments. We emphasise the role of the body in the interpretation and production of iconic signs, while demonstrating the need to take into account the Umwelt theory in the research paradigm of the experiments. Uexküll’s Umwelt theory is used to exemplify the methodological problems connected to the teaching of human language to other animal species; furthermore, we discuss how the modelling capacities of language affect the biological layer that constitutes the animal Umwelt. Language is analysed as a particular case of Umwelt transition (Tønnessen), and as such its implications are further discussed in the article. With this paper, we enrich the discussion surrounding the human-ape pidgin language by advocating for the need to include iconic components as vital parts of this research area. With this inclusion, we uncover the inter-dependency of iconic, indexical and symbolic signs in human language, aiming to further develop the research paradigm of the ape language experiments.

Keywords

Language Iconicity Apes Interspecies communication Umwelt 

References

  1. Ahlner, F., & Zlatev, J. (2010). Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies, 38(1), 298–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Augustine of Hippo (1962) [c. 397–426]. De Doctrina Christiana. In Opera, Pars IV, 1. Turholti: Editores Pontificii. —1968 [399 AD]. De Trinitate. Turnholt: Brepols.Google Scholar
  3. Beattie, G., & Shovelton, H. (1999). Do iconic hand gestures really contribute anything to the semantic information conveyed by speech? An experimental investigation. Semiotica, 123, 1–30.  https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1999.123.1-2.1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Belin, P. (2006). Voice processing in human and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 361(1476), 2091–2107.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1933.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bettoni, M. (2007). The Yerkish language - from operational methodology to chimpanzee communication. Constructivist foundations, 2(2–3), 107–121.Google Scholar
  6. Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.Google Scholar
  7. Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  8. Brakke, K. E., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1995). The development of language skills in bonobo and chimpanzee - I. Comprehension. Language and Communication, 15(2), 121–148.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(95)00001-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Brace and World.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chomsky, N. (2000). New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clarke Jr., D. S. (1987). Principles of semiotic. London: Routledge & Kegan.Google Scholar
  12. Cohen, J. (2010). Boxed about the ears, ape, language research field is still standing. Science, 2(328), 38–39.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.328.5974.38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cormier, K., Smith, S., & Sevcikova, Z. (2013). Predicate structures, gestures, and simultaneity in the representation of action in British sign language: Evidence from deaf children and adults. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18(3), 370–390.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. De Saussure, F. (1992). Course in general linguistics. LaSalle, III: Open Court.Google Scholar
  15. Deacon, W. T. (1997). The symbolic species. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
  16. Dingemanse, M. (2011). The meaning and use of ideophones in Siwu. PhD dissertation, Nijmegen: Radboud University. Available at http://thesis.ideophone.org/
  17. Duan, M. (2012). On the arbitrary nature of linguistic sign. Theory and practice in language studies, 2(1), 54–59.  https://doi.org/10.4304/tpls.2.1.54-59. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Eco, U. (1976). A theory of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ellen, R. (1977). Anatomical classifications and the semiotics of the body. In J. Blacking (Ed.), The Anthropology of the Body (pp. 343–373). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Fitch, W. T. (2017). Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 3–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165(3894), 664–672.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goldberg, E. (2018). Creativity: The human brain in the age of innovation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Hayes, C. (1951). The ape in our house. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  24. Herman, L. M. (1980). Cognitive characteristics of dolphins. In L. M. Herman (Ed.), Cetacean behavior (pp. 408–409). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Hermann, E., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes’ use of iconic cues in the object-choice task. Animal Cognition, 9(2), 118–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hill, J. H. (1980). Apes and language. In T. A. Sebeok & J. Umiker-Sebeok (Eds.), Speaking of apes. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  27. Hillix, W., & Rumbaugh, D. (2004). Animal bodies, human minds: Ape, dolphin, and parrot language skills. New York: Plenum Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. J. (1994). Sound symbolism. Cambridge: University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Hoffmeyer, J. (1996). Signs of meaning in the universe. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Holler, J., Shovelton, H., & Beattie, G. (2009). Do iconic hand gestures really contribute to the communication of semantic information in a face-to-face context? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33(2), 73–88.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-008-0063-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hubbard, E. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2001). Synaesthesia - a window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.Google Scholar
  32. Hurford, J. R. (2012). The origin of grammar: Language in the light of evolution II. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Husserl, E. (1936–1939 [1970]). The origin of geometry. In E. Husserl, The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology: An introduction to phenomenological philosophy (pp. 353–378). Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651).  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0298.
  35. Jakobson, R. (1965). Quest for the essence of language. Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 18(6), 3–5.Google Scholar
  36. Jakobson, R., & Waugh, L. R. (1979). The sound shape of language. Bloomington: Indiana Press.Google Scholar
  37. Johnson G. (1995). Chimp talk debate: is it really language? The New York Times. Newspaper article. Retrieved from http://faculty.washington.edu/losterho/nytimes%20animal%20language.pdf. Accessed 21st January 2018.
  38. Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a dome dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science, 304(5677), 1682–1683.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
  41. Kohn, E. (2005). Runa realism: Upper Amazonian attitudes to nature knowing. Ethnos, 70(2), 171–196.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00141840500141162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kohn, E. (2007). How dogs dream: Amazonian natures and the politics of transspecies engagement. American Ethnologist, 34(1), 3–24.  https://doi.org/10.1525/ae.2007.34.1.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kohn, E. (2013). How forests think: Towards an anthropology beyond the human. Berkeley: University of California Press.  https://doi.org/10.1525/california/9780520276109.001.0001. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kull, K. (1998). On semiosis, umwelt and Semiosphere. Semiotica, 120(3/4), 299–310.Google Scholar
  45. Lenneberg, H. E. (1980). Of language knowledge, apes and brains. In T. A. Sebeok & J. Umiker-Sebeok (Eds.), Speaking of apes (pp. 115–140). Boston, MA: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lestel, D. (2002). The biosemiotic and phylogenesis of culture. Social Science Information, 41(1), 35–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lestel, D. (2014). The mirror effects. Angelaki, Journal of the Theoretical Humanities, 19(3), 47–57.Google Scholar
  48. Lieberman, P. (1998). Eve spoke: Human language and human evolution. Great Britain: Picador.Google Scholar
  49. Lieberth, A. K., & Gamble, M. E. B. (1991). The role of iconicity in sign language learning by hearing adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 24(2), 89–99.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Lillo-Martin, D. (2012). Utterance reports and constructed action in sign and spoken languages. In R. Pfau, M. Steinbach, & B. Woll (Eds.), Sign language: An international handbook (pp. 365–387). Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  51. Lilly, J. C. (1967). Dolphin’s vocal mimicry as a unique ability and a step toward understanding. In K. Salzinger & S. Salzinger (Eds.), Research in verbal behavior and some neurophysiological implications (pp. 21–27). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Lord Monboddo, J. B. (1779). Of the origins and progress of language. Edinburgh: J. Baltour and T. Codell.Google Scholar
  53. Lotman, J. (1977). Primary and secondary communication modeling systems. In D. P. Lucid (Ed.), Soviet semiotics: An anthology (pp. 95–98). Baltimore & London: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., & Hopkins, W. D. (2010). The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychological Science, 21(3), 360–365.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Mandell, C., & McCabe, A. (1997). Problem of meaning behavioural and cognitive perspectives: Behavioral and cognitive perspectives. Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  56. Merrell, F. (2001). Distinctly human umwelt? Semiotica, 134(1/4), 229–262.Google Scholar
  57. Metzger, M. (1995). Constructed dialogue and constructed action in American sign language. In C. Lucas (Ed.), Sociolinguistics in deaf communities (pp. 255–271). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Miles, H. L. W. (1993). Language and the orangutan: The “old person” of the forest. In P. Cavalieri & P. Singer (Eds.), The great ape project (pp. 45–50). New York: St. Martin's.Google Scholar
  59. Morris, C. (1946). Signs, language and behavior. New York: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Nöth, W. (1995). Handbook of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Ormel, E., Hermans, D., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). The role of sign phonology and iconicity during sign processing: The case of deaf children. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(4), 436–448.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Patterson, F. G. P., & Cohn, R. H. (1990). Language acquisition by a lowland gorilla: Koko's first ten years of vocabulary development. Word, 41(2), 97–143.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1990.11435816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Patterson, F. G. P., & Linden, E. (1981). The education of Koko. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  65. Patterson, F. G. P., & Matevia, M. L. (2001). Twenty-seven years of project Koko and Michael. In B. M. F. Galdikas, N. E. Briggs, L. K. Sheeran, G. L. Shapiro, & J. Goodall (Eds.), All Apes Great and Small: African Apes (pp. 165–176). New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Publishers.Google Scholar
  66. Peirce, C. S. (1931–58). Collected writings (8 Vols.). C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss & A.W. Burks (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Pelc, J. (1986). Iconicity. Iconic signs or iconic uses of signs? In P. Bouissac, M. Herzfeld, & R. Posner (Eds.), Iconicity: Essays on the nature of culture (pp. 7–15). Tiibingen: Stauffenberg.Google Scholar
  68. Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 55(2), 139–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Pepperberg, I. M. (2006). Ordinality and inferential abilities of a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(3), 205–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Perlman, M. (2016). Research: iconic gestures. Resource Document. http://www.koko.org/research-iconic-gestures. Accessed 21st January 2018.
  71. Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1651), 1–13.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G. (2010). Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(227), 1–15.Google Scholar
  73. Persson, T. (2008). Pictorial primates: A search for iconic abilities in great apes (PhD dissertation). Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies.Google Scholar
  74. Petrilli, S., & Ponzio, A. (2015). Language as primary modeling and natural languages: A biosemiotic perspective. In E. Velmezova, K. Kull, & S. J. Cowley (Eds.), Biosemiotics perspectives on language and linguistics (pp. 47–76). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Piotr, T. (2006). From iconicity to arbitrariness: How do gestures become signs in peer-group pidgin. Psychology of Language and Communication, 10(2), 27–60.Google Scholar
  76. Quinto-Pozos, D. (2007). Why does constructed action seem obligatory? An analysis of classifiers and the lack of articulator-referent correspondence. Sign Language Studies, 7(4), 458–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Quinto-Pozos, D., Cormier, K., Ramsey, C., (2009). Constructed action of highly animate referents: evidence from American, British and Mexican Sign Languages. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Special Session on Non-Speech Modalities), Berkeley: University of California.Google Scholar
  78. Roberts, S. J. (2000). Nativization and the genesis of Hawaiian creole. In J. McWhorter (Ed.), Language change and language contact in pidgins and creoles (pp. 257–300). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Roffman, I. (2008). Are bicultural bonobos able to recognize iconic representations and produce referential signs in human cultural terms? (Master’s thesis). Iowa: Iowa State University.Google Scholar
  80. Ruthrof, H. (2010). How to get the body back into language. Corpo e Linguaggio, 2, 136–151.  https://doi.org/10.4396/20100408.Google Scholar
  81. Sandler, W. (2009). Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica, 174(1/4), 241–275.Google Scholar
  82. Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 225–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., & Boysen, S. (1980). Linguistically mediated tool use and exchange by chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes). In T. A. Sebeok & J. Umiker-Sebeok (Eds.), Speaking of apes (pp. 353–383). Boston: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., & McDonald, K. (1985). Language learning in two species of apes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 9(4), 653–665.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(85)90012-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Fields, W. M., & Taglialatela, J. (2001). Language, speech, tools and writing: A cultural imperative. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 273–292.Google Scholar
  86. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D., & Fields, W. M. (2009). Empirical Kanzi: The ape language controversy revisited. Skeptic, 15(1), 25–33.Google Scholar
  87. Schembri, A. (2003). Rethinking “classifiers” in signed languages. In K. Emmorey (Ed.), Perspectives on classifier constructions in signed languages (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  88. Schilhab, T., Stjernfelt, F., & Deacon, T. (2012). The symbolic species evolved. London and New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Sebeok, T. A. (1987). Toward a natural history of language. Semiotica, 65(3–4), 343–358.Google Scholar
  90. Sebeok, T. A., & Danesi, M. (Eds.). (1994). Encyclopaedic dictionary of semiotics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  91. Sebeok, T. A., & Danesi, M. (2000). The forms of meaning: Modeling systems theory and semiotic analysis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Sebeok, T. A., & Umiker-Sebeok, J. (Eds.). (1980). Speaking of apes. Boston, MA: Springer.Google Scholar
  93. Segerdahl, P. (2012). Human-enculturated apes: Towards a new synthesis of philosophy and comparative psychology. In L. Birke & J. Hockenhull (Eds.), Crossing boundaries: Investigating human-animal relationships (pp. 139–160). Leiden and Boston: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Singler, J. V. (1992). Nativization and pidgin/creole genesis: A reply to Bickerton. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages, 7(2), 319–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Smith, C., & Cormier, K. (2014). In or out? Spatial scale and enactment in narratives of native and non-native signing deaf children acquiring British sign language. Sign Language Studies, 14(3), 275–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Sonesson, G. (1997). The ecological foundations of iconicity. In I. Raunch & G. Garr (Eds.), Semiotics around the world: Synthesis in diversity. Proceedings of the fifth international congress of the IASS (pp. 739–742). Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin and New York.Google Scholar
  97. Sonesson, G. (2010). From mimicry to mime by way of mimesis: Reflections on a general theory of iconicity. Sign System. Studies, 38(1/4), 18–66.Google Scholar
  98. Stjernfelt, F. (2007). Diagrammatology. An investigation on the borderlines of phenomenology, ontology and semiotics. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
  99. Stoeger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva, S., Herbst, C. T., Kwon, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22(22), 2144–2148.Google Scholar
  100. Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., & Baker, L. A. (2003). Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology, 24(1), 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Taub, S. F. (2004). Language from the body: Iconicity and metaphor in American sign language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  102. Taub, S., & Galvan, D. (2001). Patterns of conceptual encoding in ASL motion descriptions. Sign Language Studies, 1(2), 175–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Terrace, H., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206(4421), 891–902.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2009). The link between form and meaning in American sign language: Lexical processing effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 550–557.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014547.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  105. Toda, S., Fogel, A., & Kawai, M. (1990). Maternal speech to three-month-old infants in the United States and Japan. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 279–294.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013775.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Tolar, T. D., Lederberg, A. R., Gokhale, S., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The development of the ability to recognizethe meaning of iconic signs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(1), 225–240.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm045.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Tønnessen, M. (2014). Umwelt trajectories. Semiotica, 198, 159–180.Google Scholar
  108. Tønnessen, M. (2015). Umwelt and language. In E. Velmezova, K. Kull, S.J. Cowley (Eds.), Biosemiotic perspectives on language and linguistics (pp.77–92). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  109. Trask, R. L. (1995). Language: The basics. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  110. Vinson, D., Thompson, R. L., Skinner, R., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). A faster path between meaning and form? Iconicity facilitates sign recognition and production in British sign language. Journal of Memory and Language, 82(1), 56–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Von Glasersfeld, E. (1978). Les chimpanzés et le langage. La Recherche, 9(92), 725–732.Google Scholar
  112. Von Glasersfeld, E. (1979). The Yerkish language for non-human primates. American. Journal of Computational Linguistics, 1(12), 1–56.Google Scholar
  113. Von Uexküll, J. (1909). Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere. Berlin: SpringerGoogle Scholar
  114. Von Uexküll, J. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men. In C. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior, New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  115. Von Uexküll, T. (1992). Introduction: The sign theory of Jakob von Uexküll. Semiotica, 89(4), 319–391.  https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1992.89.4.319(4). Google Scholar
  116. Waugh, R. L. (1983). The relevance of research into the sound shape of language for semiotics studies. In T. Barbé (Ed.), Semiotics unfolding: Proceedings of the second congress of the International Association for Semiotics Studies (pp. 1255–1262). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
  117. Winston, E. (1995). Spatial mapping in comparative discourse frames. In K. Emmorey & J. Reilly (Eds.), Language, gesture, and space (pp. 87–114). Cambridge, MA: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  118. Woll, B., & Sieratzki, J. S. (1998). Echo phonology: Signs of a link between gesture and speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(4), 531–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Zlatev, J., Madsen, E. A., Lenninger, S., Persson, T., Sayehli, S., Sonesson, G., & van de Weijer, J. (2013). Understanding communicative intentions and semiotic vehicles by children and chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 312–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Philosophy and SemioticsUniversity of TartuTartuEstonia

Personalised recommendations