Advertisement

Biosemiotics

, Volume 3, Issue 3, pp 277–287 | Cite as

Delectable Creatures and the Fundamental Reality of Metaphor: Biosemiotics and Animal Mind

  • Wendy WheelerEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

This article argues that organisms, defined by a semi-permeable membrane or skin separating organism from environment, are (must be) semiotically alert responders to environments (both Innenwelt and Umwelt). As organisms and environments complexify over time, so, necessarily, does semiotic responsiveness, or ‘semiotic freedom’. In complex environments, semiotic responsiveness necessitates increasing plasticity of discernment, or discrimination. Such judgements, in other words, involve interpretations. The latter, in effect, consist of translations of a range of sign relations which, like metaphor, are based on transfers (carryings over) of meanings or expressions from one semiotic ‘site’ to another. The article argues that what humans describe as ‘metaphor’ (and believe is something which only pertains to human speech and mind and, in essence, is ‘not real’) is, in fact, fundamental to all semiotic and biosemiotic sign processes in all living things. The article first argues that metaphor and mind are immanent in all life, and are evolutionary, and, thus, that animals certainly do have minds. Following Heidegger and then Agamben, the article continues by asking about the place of animal mind in humans, and concludes that, as a kind of ‘night science’, ‘humananimal’ mind is central to the semiotics of Peircean abduction.

Keywords

Mind Humananimal mind Abduction Metaphor Night-science 

References

  1. Adorno, T., & Horkheimer, M. (1979). Dialectic of enlightenment. Tr. J. Cumming. London: Verso.Google Scholar
  2. Agamben, G. (2004). The open: Man and animal. Tr. K. Attell. Stanford CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Auden, W. H. (1976 [1939]). In Memory of Sigmund Freud. Collected Poems, ed. E. Mendelson, London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
  4. Baluška, F. et al. (2004). Root apices as plant command centres: the unique ‘brain-like’ status of the root apex transition zone. Biologia (Bratisl.) 59 (Suppl. 13).Google Scholar
  5. Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to An Ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bateson, G., & Bateson, M. C. (1988). Angels fear: Towards an epistemology of the sacred. New York: Bantam.Google Scholar
  7. Bateson, G. (2002). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. Cresskill: Hampton.Google Scholar
  8. Bekoff, M., & Pierce, J. (2009). Wild justice: The moral lives of animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  9. Brown, S. (2009). Play: How it shapes the brain, opens the imagination, and invigorates the soul. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  10. Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  11. Damasio, A. (1995). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. London: Picador.Google Scholar
  12. Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happen: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  13. Harries-Jones, P. (1995). A recursive vision: Ecological understanding and Gregory Bateson. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
  14. Heidegger, M. (1995). The fundamental concepts of metaphysics: World, finitude, solitude. Tr. W. McNeill and N. Walker. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Hoffmeyer, J. (2008). Biosemiotics: An examination into the signs of life and the life of signs. Tr. J. Hoffmeyer and tr. and ed. Donald Favareau. Scranton and London: University of Scranton Press.Google Scholar
  16. Jacob, F. (1988). The statue within. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  17. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  18. Loye, D. (2000). Darwin’s lost theory of love. San Jose: toExcel.Google Scholar
  19. Noble, D. (2006). The music of life: Biology beyond genes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  22. Ricoeur, P. (2003). The rule of metaphor: the creation of meaning in language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  23. Shubin, N. (2008). Your inner fish: A journey into the 3.5-billion-year history of the human body. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
  24. von Uexküll, J. (1982). The theory of meaning. Semiotica, 42–1, 25–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ulanowicz, R. E. (2009). A third window: Natural life beyond Newton and Darwin. West Conshohocken: Templeton Foundation Press.Google Scholar
  26. Wordsworth, W., & Coleridge, S.T. (1968 [1802]). Lyrical Ballads. Eds. R.L. Brett and A.R. Jones. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  27. Wu, D. (1998). Romanticism: An anthology. London: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.London Metropolitan UniversityLondonEnglandUK

Personalised recommendations