Advertisement

Neuroethics

pp 1–17 | Cite as

A History of the Locked-In-Syndrome: Ethics in the Making of Neurological Consciousness, 1880-Present

  • Stephen T. Casper
Original Paper

Abstract

Extensive scholarship has described the historical and ethical imperatives shaping the emergence of the brain death criteria in the 1960s and 1970s. This essay explores the longer intellectual history that shaped theories of neurological consciousness from the late-nineteenth century to that period, and argues that a significant transformation occurred in the elaboration of those theories in the 1960s and after, the period when various disturbances of consciousness were discovered or thoroughly elaborated. Numerous historical conditions can be identified and attributed to the production of the new theories that emerged from that period-on, not least in the broader social and cultural transformations that occurred with decolonialization, pro-democracy movements, and civil and disability rights advocacy, all contexts which exerted pressures on the institutions and professions of medicine. In this telling, the discovery of the locked-in patient is thus the exploration of a transformed vision of medical patients – one that moved them from a liminal indefinite space into a firmly grounded epistemological existence – in a backdrop in which medical professionals in particular and society in general was beginning to see the body differently. With this new vision, came a relational theory of consciousness to, a reading of a body and it signs, that shifted consciousness from an internally-derived state into a relationally-constructed object. The ontology of consciousness, whatever it was, thereby became entangled with the social condition of consciousness, one that bridged the worlds of close relationship with the social movements and anxieties about personhood that emanated out of that fraught period.

Keywords

Disturbances of consciousness Ethics and medicine History of neurology History of neuroscience History of patients Minimal conscious states 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Several groups have contributed to the creation of this paper. Fernando Vidal in particular stimulated its creation through his generous invitation to the ICREA Workshop “Personhood and the Locked-In Syndrome” at the Autonomous University of Barcelona. Equally several scholars at the Richardson Seminar in the History of Psychiatry at Weil-Cornell Medical School offered generous critical observations, including Joseph J. Fins and Nicholas Schiff. Finally, scholars at the Institute for the History of Medicine at Johns Hopkins University read a complete draft of the paper and offered extensive feedback. To all, I express my appreciation.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Stephen T Casper is currently serving as an expert witness in class action litigation. He receives book royalties from Manchester University Press and the University of Rochester Press. He has been an invited speaker at professional meetings of clinicians and historians.

Conflicts of Interest

Stephen T Casper is currently serving as an expert witness in concussion litigation.

References

  1. 1.
    Plum, F., and Jerome B. Posner. 1966. Diagnosis of stupor and coma. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis Co.Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Casper, Stephen T. 2014. The neurologists: a history of a medical speciality in modern Britain, c. 1789–2000. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Todes, Daniel P. 2014. Ivan Pavlov: A Russian life in science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Philippon, Jacques, and Jacques Poirier. 2008. Joseph Babinski: A biography. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Guenther, Katja. 2015. Localization and its discontents: A genealogy of psychoanalysis and the neuro-disciplines. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Engstrom, Eric J. 2003. Clinical psychiatry in imperial Germany: A history of psychiatric practice. Ithica: Cornel. University Press.Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Jacyna, L. Stephen. 1982. Somatic theories of mind and the interests of medicine in Britain, 1850-1879. Medical History 26 (3): 233–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Jacyna, L. Stephen. 1981. The physiology of mind, the unity of nature, and the moral order in Victorian thought. The British Journal for the History of Science 14 (2): 109–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Smith, Roger. 1973. The background of physiological psychology in natural philosophy. History of Science 11 (2): 75–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Smith, Roger. 2001. Physiology and psychology, or brain and mind, in the age of C. S. Sherrington. In Bunn, G. C., Lovie, A. D. And G. D. Richards eds. Psychology in British Society: Historical essays and personal reflections. Leicester: The British psychological. Society: 223–242.Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Bacopoulous-Viau, Alexandra and Aude Fauvel eds. 2016. The Patient’s turn: Roy porter and Psychiatry’s Tales, thirty years on. Medical History 60(1): 1–1):18.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Jewson, Nicholas D. 1976. The disappearance of the sick-man from medical cosmology, 1770-1870. Sociology 10 (2): 225–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Jewson, Nicholas D. 1974. Medical knowledge and the patronage system in 18th century England. Sociology 8 (3): 369–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Burnham, John C. 2012. The death of the sick role. Social History of Medicine 25 (4): 761–776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Porter, Roy. 1985. The patient’s view. Theory and Society 14 (2): 175–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Jacyna, L. Stephen, and Stephen T. Casper. Eds. 2012. The neurological patient in History Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press, 2012.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Stiles, Anne. 2011. Popular fiction and brain science in the late nineteenth century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Killen, Andreas. 2006. Berlin Electropolis: Shock, Nerves, and German Modernity. Berkley. University of California Press.Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Leys, Ruth. 2010. How did fear become a scientific object and what kind of object is it? Representations 110 (1): 66–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Pernick, Martin S. 1999. Brain death in a cultural context. In Stuart J younger, Robert M. Arnold, and Renie Schapiro. The Definition of Death: Contemporary Controversies, 3–33. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Haan, Joost. 2013. Locked-in syndrome as depicted in literature. Progress in Brain Research 206: 19–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Richards, Robert J. 1989. Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary theories of mind and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Hale, Piers J. (2014) Political Descent: Malthus, Mutualism and the politics of evolution in Victorian England. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Jackson, John Hughlings. 1884. The Croonian lectures on evolution and dissolution of the nervous system [lecture 2]. British Medical Journal 1 (1214): 660–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Bury, Judson. 1912. Diseases of the nervous system, 86. Manchester: University Press.Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Kroker, Kenton. 2004. Epidemic encephalitis and American neurology, 1919-1940. Bulletin of the History of Medicine. 78 (1): 108–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Kroker, Kenton. 2007. The sleep of others and the transformation of sleep research. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Gavrus, Delia. 2011. Men of dreams and men of action: Neurologists, neurosurgeons, and the performance of professional identity, 1920-1950. Bulletin of the History of Medicine. 85 (1): 57–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Fulton, John Farquhar, and Percival Bailey. 1929. Tumors in the region of the third ventricle: Their diagnosis and relation to pathological sleep. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 69 (2): 145–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Economo, C.V. 1930. Sleep as a problem of localization. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 71 (3): 249–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Penfield, Wilder. 1938. The cerebral cortex in man: I. The cerebral cortex and consciousness. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 40 (3): 417–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Ransome, S.W. 1939. Somnolence caused by hypothalamic lesions in the monkey. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 41 (1): 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Dandy, W.E. 1946. The location of the conscious center in the brain: The corpus striatum. Bulletin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital. 79: 34–48.Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Meyers, Russell. 1951. Dandy’s striatal theory of the center of the consciousness: Surgical evidence and logical analysis indicating its improbability. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 65 (6): 659–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Alford, Leland B. 1950. The localization of mental functions; a new conception. Southern Medical Journal 43 (4): 262–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Brockman, Nokman W. 1946. Site of minimal lesion producing coma. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society 11 (1–2): 90–91.Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Thompson, G.N., and J.M. Nielsen. 1948. Area essential to consciousness. Cerebral localization of consciousness as established by pathological studies. Journal of the American Medical Association 137: 285.Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    MacLean, Paul D. 1977. The triune brain in conflict. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 28 (1–4): 207–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Gowers, William Richard. 1907. The Border-Land of Epilepsy: Faints, Vagal Attacks, Vertigo, Migraine, Sleep Symptoms, and their Treatment. London: P, 34. Blakiston’s Son & Co..Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Ryle, J.A. 1950. Angor animi, or the sense of dying. Guy’s Hospital Reports 99 (4): 230–235.Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    French, John D. 1952. Brain lesions associated with prolonged unconsciousness. AMA Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 68 (6): 727–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Cairns, Hugh. 1952. Disturbances of consciousness with lesions of the brain-stem and diencephalon. Brain 75 (2): 109–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Shaw, Lynsey. 2015. Neuropsychiatry and the Management of Aerial Warfare: The Royal air Force Neuropsychiatric Division in the second world war. Phd dissertation. In University of Oxford.Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Goodman, Susan. 1986. The Spirit of stoke Mandeville: The story of sir Ludwig Guttmann. London: Collins.Google Scholar
  45. 45.
    Anderson, Julie. 2003. Turned into taxpayers: Paraplegia, rehabilitation and sport at stoke Mandeville, 1944-56. Journal of Contemporary History 38 (3): 461–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. 46.
    Nicholls, Alice. 2011. Life in the Balance: Critical Illness and British Intensive Care, 1948–1986. PhD Dissertation. University of Manchester.Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Howell, Joel D. 1995. Technology in the Hospital: Transforming patient Care in the Early Twentieth Century. Baltimore and London: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Löwy, Ilana. 1996. Between bench and bedside: Science healing and Interleukin-2 in a Cancer Ward. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    Keating, Peter, and Alberto Cambrosio. 2003. Biomedical platforms: Realigning the normal and pathological in late-twentieth century medicine. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Stanton, Jennifer. 1999. Making sense of Technologies in Medicine. Social History of Medicine 12 (3): 437–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Snider, Gordon L. 1989. Historical perspective on mechanical ventilation: From simple life support system to ethical dilemma. American Review of Respiratory Disease 140 (2): S2–S7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Schaller, Walter F. 1939. After-effects of head injury: The post-traumatic concussion state (concussion, traumatic encephalopathy) and the post-traumatic psychoneurotic state (psychoneuroses, hysteria): A study in differential diagnosis. Journal of the American Medical Association 113 (20): 1779–1785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. 53.
    Cairns, Hugh. 1941. Head injuries in motor-cyclists: The importance of the crash helmet. British Medical Journal 2 (4213): 465–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Teasdale, Graham, and Bryan Jennett. 1974. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. The Lancet 304 (7872): 81–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    Jennett, Bryan, and Fred Plum. 1972. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage: A syndrome in search of a name. The Lancet 299 (7753): 734–737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    1968. A definition of irreversible coma. Report of the Ad Hoc Committee of the Harvard Medical School to Examine the Definition of Brain Death. Journal of the American Medical Association 205: 337–40.Google Scholar
  57. 57.
    Vidal, Fernando. 2009. Brainhood, anthropological figure of modernity. History of the Human Sciences 22 (1): 5–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Rose, Nikolas S., and Joelle M. Abi-Rached. 2013. Neuro: The New Brain Sciences and the Management of the Mind. Princeton. In NJ: Princeton University press.Google Scholar
  59. 59.
    Cairns, Hugh, R.C. Oldfield, J.B. Pennybacker, and D. Whitteridge. 1941. Akinetic mutism with an epidermoid cyst of the 3rd ventricle. Brain 64 (4): 273–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. 60.
    Cravioto, Humberto, Jacobo Silberman, and Irwin Feigin. 1960. A clinical and pathological study of Akinetic mutism. Neurology 10: 10–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Valenstein, E. 1975. Nonlanguage disorders of speech reflect complex neurological apparatus. Geriatrics 30 (9): 117–121.Google Scholar
  62. 62.
    Kaada, B.R., W. Harkmark, and O. Stokke. 1961. Deep coma associate with desynchronization in EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 13 (5): 785–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. 63.
    Russell, W. Ritchie. 1971. The traumatic amnesias. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  64. 64.
    Levin, Max. 1960. The mind-brain problem and Hughling Jackson’s doctrine of concomitance. American Journal of Psychiatry. 116 (8): 718–722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Hawkes, C.H. 1974. Locked-in syndrome: Report of seven cases. British Medical Journal. 4 (5941): 379–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Markand, Omkar N. 1976. Electroencephalogram in “locked-in” syndrome. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 40 (5): 529–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Hilbermand, M. 1975. The evolution of intensive care units. Critical Care Medicine 3: 159–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Reiser, Stanley J. 1992. The intensive care unit: The unfolding and ambiguities of survival therapy. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care. 8: 382–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Cappa, Stefano F., and Juigi A. Vignolo. 1982. Locked-in syndrome for 12 years with preserved intelligence. Annals of Neurology 11 (5): 545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. 70.
    Nizzi, Marie-Christine, Athena Demertzi, Olivia Gosseries, Marie-Aurélie Bruno, François Jouen, and Steven Laureys. 2012. From armchair to wheelchair: How patients with a locked-in syndrome integrate bodily changes in experienced identity. Consciousness and Cognition. 21 (1): 431–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. 71.
    Murphy, Martin J., Douglas W. Brenton, Carol A. Aschenbrener, C. John, and Van Gilder. 1979. Locked-in syndrome caused by a solitary pontine abscess. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 42 (11): 439–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Halsey, James H., and Allan H. Downie. 1966. Decerebrate rigidity with preservation of consciousness. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 29 (4): 350–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. 73.
    Allen, C.M.C. 1993. Conscious but paralyzed: Releasing the locked-in. The Lancet 342 (8864): 130–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Karz, Richard, Andrew J. Haig, Barbara B. Clarke, and Rocco J. DiPaoloa. 1992. Long-term survival, prognosis, and life-care planning for 29 patients with chronic locked-in syndrome. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 73: 403–408.Google Scholar
  75. 75.
    Doble, Jennifer E., Andrew J. Haig, Christopher Anderson, and Richard Katz. 2003. Impairment, activity, participation, life satisfaction, and survival in persons with locked-in syndrome for over a decade: Follow-up on a previously reported cohort. Journal of Head Trauma and Rehabilitation 18 (5): 435–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Birbaumer, N., N. Ghanayim, T. Hinterberger, I. Iversen, B. Kotchoubey, A. Kübler, J. Perelmouter, E. Taub, and H. Flor. 1999 Mar. A spelling device for the paralysed. Nature 398 (6725): 297–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. 77.
    Kübler, A., B. Kotchoubey, J. Kaiser, J.R. Wolpaw, and N. Birbaumer. 2001 May. Brain–computer communication: Unlocking the locked in. Psychological Bulletin 127 (3): 358–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Jacyna, L. Stephen. 2000. Lost Words: Narratives of Language and the Brain, 1825–1926. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  79. 79.
    Laureys, Steven; Pellas, Frédéric; Van Eeckhout, Philippe; Ghorbel, Sofiane, Schnakers, Caroline, Perrin, Fabien, and Jacques Berre 2005. The locked-in syndrome: What is it like to be conscious but paralyzed and voiceless? Progress in Brain Research 150: 495–511.Google Scholar
  80. 80.
    Chisholm, Nick, and Grant Gillett. 2005. The patient’s journey: Living with locked-in syndrome. British Medical Journal 331 (7508): 94–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Clarkson Honors ProgramClarkson UniversityPotsdamUSA

Personalised recommendations