Advertisement

Neuroethics

, Volume 8, Issue 2, pp 93–106 | Cite as

Reasons for Comfort and Discomfort with Pharmacological Enhancement of Cognitive, Affective, and Social Domains

  • Laura Y. CabreraEmail author
  • Nicholas S. Fitz
  • Peter B. Reiner
Original Paper

Abstract

The debate over the propriety of cognitive enhancement evokes both enthusiasm and worry. To gain further insight into the reasons that people may have for endorsing or eschewing pharmacological enhancement (PE), we used empirical tools to explore public attitudes towards PE of twelve cognitive, affective, and social (CAS) domains (e.g., attention, mood, creativity). Participants (N = 1,408) from Canada and the United States were recruited using Mechanical Turk and were randomly assigned to read one (and only one) vignette that described an individual who uses a pill to enhance a single domain. After reading the vignette, participants were asked how comfortable they were with the individual using the enhancement. People were significantly more comfortable when they read about enhancement of certain CAS domains (e.g. creativity) than others (e.g. mood). We found a modest negative correlation between comfort level and the degree to which the PE was perceived as changing core features of the person. We also found a modest correlation between comfort level and the degree to which the PE was perceived as improving success in life. Finally, using a sequential mixed method technique, we found that participants who felt uncomfortable about PE use overwhelmingly focused on a lack of need and, to a lesser degree, expressed concerns about safety; those who felt comfortable about PE use most frequently mentioned the safety of the pill and its ability to provide a positive outcome. The data provide novel insights into public enthusiasms and concerns over the use of PE.

Keywords

Enhancement Experimental neuroethics Public attitudes Cognitive Social Affective 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank Jordan Mowat for his work as a second coder.

Supplementary material

12152_2014_9222_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (166 kb)
ESM 1 (PDF 165 kb)

References

  1. 1.
    McCabe, Sean Esteban, John Knight, J.C. Teter, and H. Wechser. 2005. Non-medical use of prescription stimulants among US college students: prevalence and correlates from a national survey. Addiction 100: 96–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00944.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Teter, Christian J, Sean Esteban McCabe, Kristy LaGrange, James A Cranford, and Carol Boyd. 2006. Illicit Use of Specific Prescription Stimulants Among College Students: Prevalence, Motives, and Routes of Administration. The Journal of Pharmacotherapy 46:1501–1510.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Franke, A.G., C. Bonertz, M. Christmann, M. Huss, A. Fellgiebel, E. Hildt, and K. Lieb. 2010. Non-Medical Use of Prescription Stimulants and Illicit Use of Stimulants for Cognitive Enhancement in Pupils and Students in Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry 44: 60–66. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1268417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Castaldi, Silvana, Umberto Gelatti, Grazia Orizio, Uwe Hartung, Ana Maria Moreno-Londono, Marta Nobile, and Peter J. Schulz. 2012. Use of Cognitive Enhancement Medication Among Northern Italian University Students. Journal of Addiction Medicine 6: 112–117. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3182479584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    White, Barbara Prudhomme, Kathryn A. Becker-Blease, and Kathleen Grace-Bishop. 2006. Stimulant Medication Use, Misuse, and Abuse in an Undergraduate and Graduate Student Sample. Journal of American College Health 54: 261–268. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.5.261-268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Elliott, R., B.J. Sahakian, K. Matthews, A. Bannerjea, J. Rimmer, and T.W. Robbins. 1997. Effects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology 131: 196–206. doi: 10.1007/s002130050284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Müller, U., J.B. Rowe, T. Rittman, C. Lewis, T.W. Robbins, and B.J. Sahakian. 2013. Effects of modafinil on non-verbal cognition, task enjoyment and creative thinking in healthy volunteers. Neuropharmacology 64: 490–495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Lynch, Gary. 2002. Memory enhancement: the search for mechanism-based drugs. Nature Neuroscience 5: 1035–1038. doi: 10.1038/nn935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Gagnon, Geneviève, Cyril Schneider, Simon Grondin, and Sophie Blanchet. 2011. Enhancement of episodic memory in young and healthy adults: a paired-pulse TMS study on encoding and retrieval performance. Neuroscience Letters 488: 138–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    de Jongh, Reinoud, Ineke Bolt, Maartje Schermer, and Berend Olivier. 2008. Botox for the brain: enhancement of cognition, mood and pro-social behavior and blunting of unwanted memories. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 32: 760–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Chatterjee, A. 2004. Cosmetic neurology The controversy over enhancing movement, mentation, and mood. Neurology 63: 968–974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Elliott, C. 2000. Pursued by happiness and beaten senseless. Prozac and the American dream. The Hastings Center Report 30: 7–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Kramer, Peter D. 1993. Listening to Prozac. New York: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Bostrom, Nick, and Anders Sandberg. 2009. Cognitive Enhancement: Methods, Ethics, Regulatory Challenges. Science and Engineering Ethics 15: 311–341. doi: 10.1007/s11948-009-9142-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Fukuyama, Francis. 2003. Our Posthuman Future. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    President's Council on Bioethics. 2003. Beyond Therapy. Washington: President's Council on Bioethics.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Savulescu, Julian. 2014. The ethics of genetically enhanced monkey-slaves. Edited by David Webber. TED blog.Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Eickenhorst, Patrick, Karin Vitzthum, F. Burghard, David Groneberg Klapp, and Stefanie Mache. 2012. Neuroenhancement among German university students: motives, expectations, and relationship with psychoactive lifestyle drugs. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 44: 418–427. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2012.736845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Fitz, Nicholas S., Roland Nadler, Praveena Manogaran, Eugene W.J. Chong, and Peter B. Reiner. 2014. Public Attitudes Toward Cognitive Enhancement. Neuroethics 7: 173–188. doi: 10.1007/s12152-013-9190-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Forlini, Cynthia, and Eric Racine. 2012. Added Stakeholders, Added Value(s) to the Cognitive Enhancement Debate: Are Academic Discourse and Professional Policies Sidestepping Values of Stakeholders? AJOB Primary Research 3: 33–47. doi: 10.1080/21507716.2011.645116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Franke, Andreas G., Klaus Lieb, and Elisabeth Hildt. 2012. What Users Think about the Differences between Caffeine and Illicit/Prescription Stimulants for Cognitive Enhancement. PloS One 7: e40047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Sattler, Sebastian, Guido Mehlkop, Peter Graeff, and Carsten Sauer. 2014. Evaluating the drivers of and obstacles to the willingness to use cognitive enhancement drugs: the influence of drug characteristics, social environment, and personal characteristics. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy 9: 8. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-9-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Schelle, Kimberly J., Nadira Faulmüller, Lucius Caviola, and Miles Hewstone. 2014. Attitudes toward pharmacological cognitive enhancement—a review. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 8: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Bell, S., Brad Partridge, Jayne C. Lucke, and Wayne Hall. 2013. Australian University Students' Attitudes Towards the Acceptability and Regulation of Pharmaceuticals to Improve Academic Performance. Neuroethics 6: 197–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Bergstrom, Lena, and Niels Lynoe. 2008. Enhancing concentration, mood and memory in healthy individuals: An empirical study of attitudes among general practitioners and the general population. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 36: 532–537. doi: 10.1177/1403494807087558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Scheske, Christel, and Simone Schnall. 2012. The Ethics of “Smart Drugs”: Moral Judgments About Healthy People's Use of Cognitive-Enhancing Drugs. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 34: 508–515. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2012.711692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Asscher, Eva C.A., Ineke Bolt, and Maartje Schermer. 2012. Wish-fulfilling medicine in practice: a qualitative study of physician arguments. Journal of Medical Ethics 38: 327–331. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2011-100103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Forlini, Cynthia, and Eric Racine. 2009. Autonomy and Coercion in Academic “Cognitive Enhancement” Using Methylphenidate: Perspectives of Key Stakeholders. Neuroethics 2: 163–177. doi: 10.1007/s12152-009-9043-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Maier, Larissa J., E. Matthias, Fiona Herzig Liechti, and Michael P. Schaub. 2013. To Dope or Not to Dope: Neuroenhancement with Prescription Drugs and Drugs of Abuse among Swiss University Students. PloS One 8: e77967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Dodge, Tonya, J. Kevin, Miesha Marzell Williams, and Rob Turrisi. 2012. Judging cheaters: Is substance misuse viewed similarly in the athletic and academic domains? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 26: 678–682. doi: 10.1037/a0027872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Dubljević, V. 2013. Cognitive Enhancement, Rational Choice and Justification. Neuroethics 6: 179–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Sabini, John, and John Monterosso. 2005. Judgments of the Fairness of Using Performance Enhancing Drugs. Ethics & Behavior 15: 81–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb1501_6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Banjo, Opeyemi C., Roland Nadler, and Peter B. Reiner. 2010. Physician Attitudes towards Pharmacological Cognitive Enhancement: Safety Concerns Are Paramount. PloS One 5: e14322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014322.t003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Riis, Jason, and Joseph P. Simmons. 2008. Preferences for Enhancement Pharmaceuticals: The Reluctance to Enhance Fundamental Traits. Journal of Consumer Research 35: 495–508. doi: 10.1086/588746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Degrazia, David. 2005. Enhancement Technologies and Human Identity. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 30: 261–283. doi: 10.1080/03605310590960166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Levy. 2007. Neuroethics. Cambridge University: Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Bolt, Ineke, and Maartje Schermer. 2009. Psychopharmaceutical Enhancers: Enhancing Identity? Neuroethics 2: 103–111. doi: 10.1007/s12152-008-9031-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Focquaert, Farah, and Dirk De Ridder. 2009. Direct intervention in the brain: ethical issues concerning personal identity. Journal of Ethics in Mental Health 4: 1–7.Google Scholar
  39. 39.
    Brey, P. 2009. Human enhancement and personal identity. In New Waves in Philosophy of Technology., eds. B. Olsen, J. Selinger, and E. Riis, 169–185. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Witt, Karsten, Jens Kuhn, Lars Timmermann, Mateusz Zurowski, and Christiane Woopen. 2011. Deep Brain Stimulation and the Search for Identity. Neuroethics 6: 499–511. doi: 10.1007/s12152-011-9100-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Forlini, Cynthia, and Eric Racine. 2011. Considering the Causes and Implications of Ambivalence in Using Medicine for Enhancement. The American Journal of Bioethics 11: 15–17. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2011.534952.
  42. 42.
    Aikins, Ross D. 2011. Academic Performance Enhancement: A Qualitative Study of the Perceptions and Habits of Prescription Stimulant–Using College Students. Journal of College Student Development 52: 560–576. doi: 10.1353/csd.2011.0064.
  43. 43.
    Burstin, Kenneth, Eugene B. Doughtie, and Avi Raphaeli. 1980. Contrastive Vignette Technique: An indirect Methodology Designed to Address Reactive Social Attitude Measurement1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 10: 147–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1980.tb00699.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Knobe, Joshua, and Bertram F. Malle. 2002. Self and other in the explanation of behavior: 30 years later. Psychologica Belgica 42: 113–130.Google Scholar
  45. 45.
    Willis, Gordon. 2004. Cognitive interviewing. Research Triangle Institute.Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Ipeirotis, Panagiotis G. 2010. Demographics of Mechanical Turk.Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Paolacci, Gabriele, Jesse Chandler, and Panagiotis G. Ipeirotis. 2009. Running experiments on amazon mechanical turk. Judgment and Decision Making 5: 411–419.Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Buhrmester, Michael, Tracy Kwang, and Samuel D. Gosling. 2011. Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data? Perspectives on Psychological Science 6: 3–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Berinsky, Adam J., Gregory A. Huber, and Gabriel S. Lenz. 2012. Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis 20: 351–368. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpr057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. 50.
    Mason, Winter, and Siddharth Suri. 2012. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods 44: 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    McCrae, Robert R., and Paul T. Costa. 1987. Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52: 81–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    McCrae, Robert R, Antonio Terracciano, Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. 2005. Universal Features of Personality Traits From the Observer's Perspective: Data From 50 Cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 547–561. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547.
  53. 53.
    McCrae, Robert R., and Oliver P. John. 1992. An Introduction to the Five-Factor Model and Its Applications. Journal of Personality 60: 175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    DeYoung, Colin G., Lena Quilty, and Jordan Peterson. 2007. Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93: 880–896. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    Matthews, Gerald, Ian J. Deary, and Martha C. Whiteman. 2009. Personality Traits. Cambridge University: Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    Kolber, Adam. 2006. Therapeutic Forgetting: The Legal and Ethical Implications of Memory Dampening. Vanderbilt law review 59: 1561–1626.Google Scholar
  57. 57.
    Sandberg, A., and N. Bostrom. 2006. Converging Cognitive Enhancements. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1093: 201–227. doi: 10.1196/annals.1382.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Lin, Patrick, and Fritz Allhoff. 2008. Against unrestricted human enhancement. Journal of Evolution and Technology 18: 35.Google Scholar
  59. 59.
    Savulescu, Julian, Ruud ter Meulen, and Guy Kahane. 2011. Enhancing Human Capacities. John Wiley &: Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. 60.
    Watts, Susan. 2011. The dope on mental enhancement. November: New Scientist. 15.Google Scholar
  61. 61.
    Petrow, Steven. 2014. The Drugs of Work-Performance Enhancement. February: The Atlantic. 24.Google Scholar
  62. 62.
    Cumming, G. 2014. The New Statistics Why and How. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
  63. 63.
    Cabrera, Laura Y. and Peter B. Reiner. (submitted). A novel sequential mixed-method technique for quantification of unscripted narratives: Contrastive quantitized content analysis: 1–18.Google Scholar
  64. 64.
    Chi, Michelene T.H. 1997. Quantifying qualitative analyses of verbal data: A practical guide. The Journal of the Learning Sciences 6: 271–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Braun, Virginia, and Victoria Clarke. 2006. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3: 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Lombard, Matthew, Jennifer Snyder-Duch, and Cheryl Campanella Bracken. 2002. Content Analysis in Mass Communication: Assessment and Reporting of Intercoder Reliability. Human Communication Research 28: 587–604. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Sandelowski, Margarete, Corrine I. Voils, and G. Knafl. 2009. On Quantitizing. Journal of Mixed Methods Research 3: 208–222. doi: 10.1177/1558689809334210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Onwuegbuzie, Anthony J. 2003. Effect sizes in qualitative research: A prolegomenon. Quality and Quantity 37: 393–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Paolacci, Gabriele, and Jesse Chandler. 2014. Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a Participant Pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science 23: 184–188. doi: 10.1177/0963721414531598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. 70.
    Howden, Lindsay M., and Julie A. Meyer. 2011. Age and Sex Composition: 2010. US Census Bureau 1–16.Google Scholar
  71. 71.
    Bostrom, Nick, and Toby Ord. 2006. The reversal test: eliminating status quo bias in applied ethics. Ethics 116: 656–679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Park, Nansook, Christopher Peterson, and M.E.P. Seligman. 2004. Strengths of Character and Well-Being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 23: 603–619. doi: 10.1521/jscp.23.5.603.50748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. 73.
    Yearley, Lee H. 1990. Mencius and Aquinas. SUNY: Press.Google Scholar
  74. 74.
    Hughes, James J. 2013. Using Neurotechnologies to Develop Virtues: A Buddhist Approach to Cognitive Enhancement. Accountability in Research: Policies and Quality 20: 27–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. 75.
    MacLean, Evan L., Brian Hare, L. Charles, Elsa Addessi Nunn, Federica Amici, C. Rindy, Filippo Aureli Anderson, et al. 2014. The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academic of Sciences 111: E2140–E2148. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323533111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Mischel, Walter, Yuichi Shoda, and M.L. Rodriguez. 1989. Delay of Gratification in Children. Science 244: 933–938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. 77.
    Moffitt, Terrie E., Louise Arseneault, Daniel Belsky, Nigel Dickson, J. Robert, Honalee Harrington Hancox, Renate Houts, et al. 2011. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Tangney, June P., R.F. Baumeister, and A.L. Boone. 2004. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality 72: 271–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. 79.
    Peterson, Christopher, and M.E.P. Seligman. 2004. Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook and Classification. Oxford University: Press.Google Scholar
  80. 80.
    Schermer, Maartje. 2008. Enhancements, easy shortcuts, and the richness of human activities. Bioethics 22: 355–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8519.2008.00657.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. 81.
    Bublitz, Jan Christoph, and Reinhard Merkel. 2009. Autonomy and authenticity of enhanced personality traits. Bioethics 23: 360–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8519.2009.01725.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. 82.
    Parens, Erik. 2005. Authenticity and ambivalence: toward understanding the enhancement debate. The Hastings Center Report 35: 34–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. 83.
    Levy, Neil. 2011. Enhancing Authenticity. Journal of Applied Philosophy 28: 308–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5930.2011.00532.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. 84.
    James, William. 1891. The Principles of Psychology. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  85. 85.
    Markus, Hazel. 1977. Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35: 63–78. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.35.2.63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. 86.
    Haslam, Nick, Brock Bastian, and Melanie Bissett. 2004. Essentialist beliefs about personality and their implications. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30: 1661–1673. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. 87.
    Chen, Serena, Helen C. Boucher, and M.P. Tapias. 2006. The Relational Self Revealed: Integrative Conceptualization and Implications for Interpersonal Life. Psychological Bulletin 132: 151–179. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. 88.
    Oyserman, D, K Elmore, and George Smith. 2012. Self, Self-concept and Identity. In Handbook of Self and Identity, eds. M. R. Leary and J. P. Tangney, 69–104. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  89. 89.
    Sattler, Sebastian, Carsten Sauer, Guido Mehlkop, and Peter Graeff. 2013. The Rationale for Consuming Cognitive Enhancement Drugs in University Students and Teachers. PLoS ONE 8: e68821. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068821.s003.
  90. 90.
    Franke, Andreas G., Carolin Papenburg, Elena Schotten, P.B. Reiner, and K. Lieb. 2014. Attitudes towards prescribing cognitive enhancers among primary care physicians in Germany. BMC Family Practice 15: 3. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-15-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. 91.
    Schuijff, Mirjam, and F.W. Brom. 2013. The dynamics of citizen deliberation regarding human enhancement in the Netherlands. In Beyond Therapy v. Enhancement? Multidisciplinary Analysis of a Heated Debate, ed. F. Lucivero and A. Vedder, 143–161. Pisa: Pisa University Press.Google Scholar
  92. 92.
    Sattler, S., and C. Wiegel. 2012. Cognitive Test Anxiety and Cognitive Enhancement: The Influence of Students' Worries on Their Use of Performance-Enhancing Drugs. Substance Use & Misuse 48: 220–232. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2012.751426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. 93.
    Judson, Rachel, and S.W. Langdon. 2009. Illicit use of prescription stimulants among college students: Prescription status, motives, theory of planned behaviour, knowledge and self-diagnostic tendencies. Psychology, Health & Medicine 14: 97–104. doi: 10.1080/13548500802126723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. 94.
    Lyubomirsky, Sonja, Laura King, and Ed. Diener. 2005. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin 131: 803–855. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. 95.
    Oishi, Shigehiro, Ed. Diener, and R.E. Lucas. 2007. The Optimum Level of Well-Being: Can People Be Too Happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science 2: 346–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00048.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. 96.
    Reiner, P B. 2013. The Biopolitics of Cognitive Enhancement. In Cognitive Enhancement: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, eds. E. Hildt and A. G. Franke, 189–200. Trends in Augmentation of Human Performance 1. Springer.Google Scholar
  97. 97.
    Mill, John Stuart. 1863. On Liberty. Boston: Ticknor and Fields.Google Scholar
  98. 98.
    Farah, Martha J., Judy Illes, Robert Cook-Deegan, Howard Gardner, Eric Kandel, Patricia King, Eric Parens, Barbara Sahakian, and Paul Root Wolpe. 2004. Neurocognitive enhancement: what can we do and what should we do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 421–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. 99.
    Greely, Henry, Barbara Sahakian, John Harris, C. Ronald, Michael Gazzaniga Kessler, J.M. Farah, and P. Campbell. 2008. Towards responsible use of cognitive-enhancing drugs by the healthy. Nature 456: 702–705. doi: 10.1038/456702a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. 100.
    Kaebnick, Gregory E. 2008. Nature, human nature, and biotechnology.Google Scholar
  101. 101.
    Dworkin, Ronald. 2000. Playing God: genes, clones, and luck. In Sovereign Virtue: The Theory and Practice of Equality. Harvard University Press.  Google Scholar
  102. 102.
    Daniels, N. 1992. Growth Hormone Therapy for short stature: can we support the treatment/enhancement distinction. Growth: Genetics & Hormones 8: 46–48.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laura Y. Cabrera
    • 1
    Email author
  • Nicholas S. Fitz
    • 1
  • Peter B. Reiner
    • 1
  1. 1.National Core for NeuroethicsThe University of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations