Advertisement

Striving to protect friends and family or holding everyone accountable: Moral expansiveness explains the difference between conservatives and liberals

  • Reina TakamatsuEmail author
Article
  • 12 Downloads

Abstract

The current research aimed to explain how conservatives and liberals differ in moral expansiveness, which is the extent to which an individual ascribes moral status to others. Using two measures of moral expansiveness, we demonstrated that both conservatives and liberals give moral regard to in-groups (e.g., friends, family, co-workers), but liberals tend to show more concern for out-groups (e.g., foreign citizen, people with different religious beliefs, members of LGBTI community) than conservatives do. Conservatives reserve their moral concern for friends and family, whereas liberals have no moral boundaries between people. In policy-making, conservatives and liberals disagree when their focuses of moral consideration do not match. We discuss that conflicts between the two can be explained by expansiveness of moral regard for out-groups. The results explain the relationship between political orientation and moral expansiveness and the way an individual includes others in the circle of moral regard.

Keywords

Moral expansiveness Political orientation Attitudes toward out-groups 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

There is no potential conflict of interest pertaining to this submission to Current Psychology.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

12144_2020_615_MOESM1_ESM.docx (32 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 32 kb)

References

  1. Aquino, K., & Reed, A. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.6.1423.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ariely, G. (2017). Global identification, xenophobia and globalization: A cross-national exploration. International Journal of Psychology, 52, 87–96.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12364.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bloom, P. (2016). Against empathy: The case for rational compassion. London: The Bodley Head.Google Scholar
  4. Crimston, D., Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., & Bastian, B. (2016). Moral expansiveness: Examining variability in the extension of the moral world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 636–653.  https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000086.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Crimston, D., Hornsey, M. J., Bain, P. G., & Bastian, B. (2018). Toward a psychology of moral expansiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 14–19.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417730888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cuddy, A. J. C., Rock, M., & Norton, M. I. (2007). Aid in the aftermath of hurricane Katrina: Inferences of secondary emotions and intergroup helping. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 107–118.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207071344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Day, M. V., Fiske, S. T., Downing, E. L., & Trail, T. E. (2014). Shifting liberal and conservative attitudes using moral foundations theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 1559–1573.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214551152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35, 337–358.  https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029–1046.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
  13. Hall, D. L., Cohen, A. B., Meyer, K. K., Varley, A. H., & Brewer, G. A. (2015). Costly signaling increases trust, even across religious affiliations. Psychological Science, 26, 1368–1376.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615576473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17, 847–853.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hasson, Y., Tamir, M., Brahms, K. S., Cohrs, J. C., & Halperin, E. (2018). Are liberals and conservatives equally motivated to feel empathy toward others? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 1449–1459.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218769867.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Houck, S. C., & Repke, M. (2017). When and why we torture: A review of psychology research. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 3, 272–283.  https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9, 435–439.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015960.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61, 651–670.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 126–136.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00070.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163600.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Kugler, M., Jost, J. T., & Noorbaloochi, S. (2014). Another look at moral foundations theory: Do authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain liberal-conservative differences in “moral” intuitions? Social Justice Research, 27, 413–431.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-014-0223-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Laham, S. M. (2009). Expanding the moral circle: Inclusion and exclusion mindsets and the circle of moral regard. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 250–253.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 52, 1258–1265.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my ingroup: A measure and studies of identification with all humanity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 830–853.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028724.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Muller, J. Z. (1997). What is conservative social and political thought? In J. Z. Muller (Ed.), Conservativism: An anthology of social and political thought from David Hume to the present (pp. 3–31). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 1–20.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00268.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pettigrew, T. F., Wagner, U., & Christ, O. (2007). Who opposes immigration? Comparing German with north American findings. Du Bois Review, 4, 19–39.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1742058X07070038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Reed II, A., & Aquino, K. F. (2003). Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral regard toward out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1270–1286.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Reese, G., Proch, J., & Finn, C. (2015). Identification with all humanity: The role of self-definition and self-investment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 426–440.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Reysen, S., & Hackett, J. (2015). Further examination of the factor structure and validity of the identification with all humanity scale. Current Psychology, 1–13 (Online first).  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9341-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Singer, P. (2011). The expanding circle: Ethics, evolution, and moral progress. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Skitka, L., & Tetlock, P. E. (1993). Providing public assistance: Cognitive and motivational processes underlying liberal and conservative policy preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1205–1224.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Smith, I. H., Aquino, K., Koleva, S., & Graham, J. (2014). The moral ties that bind…even to out-groups: The interactive effect of moral identity and the binding moral foundations. Psychological Science, 25, 1554–1562.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614534450.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Tang, T. L., & Tzeng, J. Y. (1992). Demographic correlates of the Protestant work ethic. Journal of Psychology, 126, 163–170.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1992.10543351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Terrizzi Jr., J. A., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587–592.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Graduate School of EducationKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations