Advertisement

Are neo-liberals more intuitive? Undetected libertarians confound the relation between analytic cognitive style and economic conservatism

  • Onurcan YilmazEmail author
  • S. Adil Saribay
  • Ravi Iyer
Article

Abstract

Previous studies consistently showed that analytic cognitive style (ACS) is negatively correlated with social conservatism, but there are mixed findings concerning its relation with economic conservatism. Most tests have relied on a unidimensional (liberal-conservative) operationalization of political orientation. Libertarians tend not only to identify themselves as conservative on this scale but also to score higher on ACS than liberals and conservatives. The presence of libertarians might be the reason for the above-mentioned mixed findings. We investigated the relation between social and economic conservatism and ACS (operationalized using the Cognitive Reflection Test; CRT) in a large, web-based sample. There was a negative correlation between CRT and social conservatism both when libertarians were included and excluded. However, the correlation between CRT and economic conservatism was significantly reduced in magnitude and became non-significant when libertarians were excluded. The results support the argument that the undetected presence of libertarians may confound the ACS-economic conservatism relation.

Keywords

Analytic cognitive style Libertarians Liberals Conservatives Cognitive reflection test 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Gordon Pennycook and Thomas Talhelm for their helpful comments.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Bahçekapili, H. G., & Yilmaz, O. (2017). The relation between different types of religiosity and analytic cognitive style. Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 267–272.Google Scholar
  2. Baron, J. (2015). Supplement to Deppe et al. (2015). Judgment and Decision making, 10(4), 1–2.Google Scholar
  3. Baron, J. (2017). Comment on Kahan and Corbin: Can polarization increase with actively open-minded thinking? Research & Politics, 4(1), 1–4.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168016688122.Google Scholar
  4. Baron, J., Scott, S., Fincher, K., & Metz, S. E. (2015). Why does the cognitive reflection test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(3), 265–284.Google Scholar
  5. Białek, M., & De Neys, W. (2016). Conflict detection during moral decision-making: Evidence for deontic reasoners’ utilitarian sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 631–639.Google Scholar
  6. Bialek, M., & Pennycook, G. (2017). The cognitive reflection test is robust to multiple exposures. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 1–7.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0963-x.Google Scholar
  7. Boaz, D., & Kirby, D. (2006). The libertarian vote. Cato Institute Policy Analysis, 580, 1–26.Google Scholar
  8. Brandt, M. J., Evans, A. M., & Crawford, J. T. (2015). The unthinking or confident extremist? Political extremists are more likely than moderates to reject experimenter generated anchors. Psychological Science, 26(2), 189–202.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Deppe, K. D., Gonzalez, F. J., Neiman, J. L., Jacobs, C., Pahlke, J., Smith, K. B., & Hibbing, J. R. (2015). Reflective liberals and intuitive conservatives: A look at the cognitive reflection test and ideology. Judgment and Decision making, 10(4), 314–331.Google Scholar
  11. Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., Goodman, J. A., & Blanchar, J. C. (2012). Low-effort thought promotes political conservatism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(6), 808–820.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 454–459.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337–358.Google Scholar
  15. Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42.Google Scholar
  16. Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science, 336(6080), 493–496.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Greenberg, J., & Jonas, E. (2003). Psychological motives and political orientation—The left, the right, and the rigid: Comment on Jost et al. (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 376–382.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Haigh, M. (2016). Has the standard cognitive reflection test become a victim of its own success? Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 12(3), 145–149.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Iyer, R., Koleva, S., Graham, J., Ditto, P., & Haidt, J. (2012). Understanding libertarian morality: the psychological dispositions of selfidentified libertarians. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42366.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61(7), 651–670.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Jost, J. T., Sterling, J., & Stern, C. (2017). Getting closure on conservatism, or the politics of epistemic and existential motivation. In C. Kopetz, & A. Fishbach (Eds.), The motivation-cognition interface; From the lab to the real world: A Festschrift in honor of Arie W. Kruglanski.Google Scholar
  22. Kahan, D. M. (2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision making, 8(4), 407–424.Google Scholar
  23. Klein, C. (2011). The dual track theory of moral decision-making: A critique of the neuroimaging evidence. Neuroethics, 4(2), 143–162.Google Scholar
  24. Landy, J. F. (2016). Representations of moral violations: Category members and associated features. Judgment and Decision making, 11(5), 496–508.Google Scholar
  25. Meyer, A., Zhou, E., & Shane, F. (2018). The non-effects of repeated exposure to the Cognitive Reflection Test. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(3), 246–259.Google Scholar
  26. Morewedge, C. K., & Kahneman, D. (2010). Associative processes in intuitive judgment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 435–440.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2013). The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 20–25.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2018). Cognitive reflection and the 2018 U.S. Presidential Election. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Google Scholar
  29. Pennycook, G., & Ross, R. M. (2016). Commentary: Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 9.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00009.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123(3), 335–346.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cognitive Psychology, 80, 34–72.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Pennycook, G., Ross, R. M., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2016). Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers: Four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS One, 11(4), e0153039.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Piazza, J., & Sousa, P. (2014). Religiosity, political orientation, and consequentialist moral thinking. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 334–342.Google Scholar
  34. Sanchez, C., Sundermeier, B., Gray, K., & Calin-Jageman, R. J. (2017). Direct replication of Gervais & Norenzayan (2012) no evidence that analytic thinking decreases religious belief. PLoS One, 12(2), e0172636.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Saribay, S. A., & Yilmaz, O. (2017). Analytic cognitive style and cognitive ability differentially predict religiosity and social conservatism. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 24–29.Google Scholar
  36. Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in god. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 423–428.Google Scholar
  37. Sinayev, A., & Peters, E. (2015). Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 532.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Skitka, L. J. (2010). The psychology of moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(4), 267–281.Google Scholar
  39. Sterling, J., Jost, J. T., & Pennycook, G. (2016). Are neoliberals more susceptible to bullshit? Judgment and Decision making, 11(4), 352–360.Google Scholar
  40. Stieger, S., & Reips, U. (2016). A limitation of the cognitive reflection test: Familiarity. PeerJ, 4, e2395.  https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2395.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Talhelm, T. (2018). Hong Kong liberals are weird: Analytic thought increases support for liberal policies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(5), 717–728.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Talhelm, T., Haidt, J., Oishi, S., Zhang, X., Miao, F. F., & Chen, S. (2015). Liberals think more analytically (more “WEIRD”) than conservatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(2), 250–267.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Thompson, V. A. (2009). Dual-process theories: A metacognitive perspective. In J. St, B. T. Evans, & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 171–196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision making, 11(1), 99–113.Google Scholar
  45. Trémolière, B., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2014). Efficient kill–save ratios ease up the cognitive demands on counterintuitive moral utilitarianism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(7), 923–930.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Van Berkel, L., Crandall, C. S., Eidelman, S., & Blanchar, J. C. (2015). Hierarchy, dominance, and deliberation egalitarian values require mental effort. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(9), 1207–1222.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Van Hiel, A., Onraet, E., & De Pauw, S. (2010). The relationship between social-cultural attitudes and behavioral measures of cognitive style: A meta-analytic integration of studies. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1765–1800.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Yilmaz, O., Karadöller, D. Z., & Sofuoglu, G. (2016). Analytic thinking, religion, and prejudice: an experimental test of the dual-process model of mind. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26(4), 360–369.Google Scholar
  49. Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2016). An attempt to clarify the link between cognitive style and political ideology: a non-western replication and extension. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(3), 287–300.Google Scholar
  50. Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2017a). Analytic thought training promotes liberalism on contextualized (but not stable) political opinions. Social Psychological and Personality Science.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616687092.
  51. Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2017b). The relationship between cognitive style and political orientation depends on the measures used. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(2), 140–147.Google Scholar
  52. Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2017c). Activating analytic thinking enhances the value given to individualizing moral foundations. Cognition, 165, 88–96.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2018). Lower levels of resistance to change (but not opposition to equality) is related to analytic cognitive style. Social Psychology, 49, 65–75.Google Scholar
  54. Yonker, J. E., Edman, L. R., Cresswell, J., & Barrett, J. L. (2016). Primed analytic thought and religiosity: The importance of individual characteristics. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 8(4), 298–308.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyDogus UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  2. 2.Bogaziçi UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  3. 3.

Personalised recommendations