Advertisement

An experimental study of the effect of rumination processing modes on approach behavior in a task involving previous failure

  • Kohei KambaraEmail author
  • Yugo Kira
  • Akiko Ogata
Article
  • 17 Downloads

Abstract

Previous studies have found that rumination involves thoughts that usually occur during and influence affect after experiencing failure. However, little is known about how rumination directly affects approach behavior beyond that point. Therefore, we investigated the effects of rumination on approach behavior after failure. Forty-nine university students were allocated to one of either an abstract or concrete processing mode of rumination or a control condition. A failure experience was induced through a creative thinking task, and rumination was either manipulated or not depending on the group. The students then performed a task that was similar to the failure-inducing creative thinking task. Our analysis revealed that only the concrete processing mode of rumination improved participants’ negative affect and that the manipulation of rumination ultimately did not influence approach behavior after experiencing failure.

Keywords

Rumination Approach behavior Processing modes Depression 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by grant from the Hiroshima University Education and Research Support Foundation. The authors thank all of their colleagues who assisted in this study. We are grateful to Dr. Natalie J Ciarocco for her advice in this study and providing us some materials.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  2. Beilock, S. L., & Gonso, S. (2008). Putting in the mind versus putting on the green: Expertise, performance time, and the linking of imagery and action. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 920–932.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701625626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2016). Self-regulation of action and affect. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 3–23). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  4. Ciarocco, N. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). Some good news about rumination: Task-focused thinking after failure facilitates performance improvement. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 1057–1073.  https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.10.1057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Connolly, S. L., & Alloy, L. B. (2017). Rumination interacts with life stress to predict depressive symptoms: An ecological momentary assessment study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 97, 86–95.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.07.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Eftekhari, E., Tran, A., & McGregor, I. (2017). Decentering increases approach motivation among distressed individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 119, 236–241.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Ehring, T., Szeimies, A. K., & Schaffrick, C. (2009). An experimental analogue study into the role of abstract thinking in trauma-related rumination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 285–293.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hasegawa, A. (2013). Translation and initial validation of the Japanese version of the ruminative responses scale 1. Psychological Reports, 112, 716–726.  https://doi.org/10.2466/02.08.PR0.112.3.716-726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Jacobson, N., & Newman, M. (2014). Avoidance mediates the relationship between anxiety and depression over a decade later. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 437–445.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kambara, K., Ogata, A., & Kira, Y. (2017). Effects of rumination processing modes on the tendency to avoid failure. Current Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9655-z.
  11. Kingston, R. E. F., Watkins, E. R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2014). Investigating functional properties of depressive rumination: Insight and avoidance. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 5, 244–258.  https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.038013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kojima, M., & Furukawa, T. (2003). Japanese version of the Beck depression inventory (2nd ed.). Tokyo: Nippon Hyoronsha.Google Scholar
  13. Koppe, K., & Rothermund, K. (2017). Let it go: Depression facilitates disengagement from unattainable goals. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 278–284.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.10.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 138–145.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Krieger, T., & Holtforth, M. (2013). Self-compassion in depression: Associations with depressive symptoms, rumination, and avoidance in depressed outpatients. Behavior Therapy, 44, 501–513.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kulkarni, A. A., & Yuan, H. (2017). Construal-level mindsets enhance behavioral persistence in response to incentive valence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making.  https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2020.
  17. Martel, C. R., Addis, M. E., & Jacobson, N. S. (2001). Depression in context: Strategies for guided action. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  18. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1–47). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2011). Unfulfilled goals interfere with tasks that require executive functions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 300–311.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing mode influences the relationship between trait rumination and emotional vulnerability. Behavior Therapy, 37, 281–291.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ottenbreit, N. D., & Dobson, K. S. (2004). Avoidance and depression: The construction of the cognitive-behavioral avoidance scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 293–313.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00140-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673–1681.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Russo, S. J., & Nestler, E. J. (2013). The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 1–34.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3381.Google Scholar
  24. Sato, T., & Yasuda, A. (2001). Development of the Japanese version of positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) scales. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 9, 138–139.  https://doi.org/10.2132/jjpjspp.9.2_138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Segerstrom, S. C., Stanton, A. L., Alde, L. E., & Shortridge, B. E. (2003). A multidimensional structure for repetitive thought: What’s on your mind, and how, and how much? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 909–921.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Shudo, Y., Yamamoto, T., & Sakai, M. (2016). Longitudinal predictions of depression symptoms using the activation and avoidance subscales of the Japanese behavioral activation scale-short form. Psychological Reports, 120, 130–140.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294116680794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Stinson, C. H., Milbrath, C., Reidbord, S. P., & Bucci, W. (1994). Thematic segmentation of psychotherapy transcripts for convergent analysis. Psychotherapy, 31, 36–48.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.31.1.36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Stöber, J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2002). Reduced concreteness of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: Findings from a therapy study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 89–96.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013845821848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Takagaki, K., Okajima, I., Nakajima, S., Ishikawa, S., Kunisato, Y., Kanai, Y., & Sakano, Y. (2013). Preliminary assessment of the behavioral activation model in Japanese undergraduate students. Psychological Reports, 112, 47–59.  https://doi.org/10.2466/02.15.21.PR0.112.1.47-59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Takahashi, Y., Yamagata, S., Kijima, N., Shigemasu, K., Ono, Y., & Ando, J. (2007). Gray’s temperament model: Development of Japanese version of BIS/BAS scales and a behavior genetic investigation using the twin method. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 15, 276–289.  https://doi.org/10.2132/personality.15.276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Watkins, E. (2004). Appraisals and strategies associated with rumination and worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 679–694.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Watkins, E. R. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action identification across psychological disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 260–278.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319–328.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Watkins, E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2014). A habit-goal framework of depressive rumination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 24–34.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Watkins E. R., Moberly N. J., &, Moulds M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8, 364–378. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364.
  38. Zoccola, P. M., Rabideau, E. M., Figueroa, W. S., & Woody, A. (2014). Cardiovascular and affective consequences of ruminating on a performance stressor depend on mode of thought. Stress and Health, 30, 188–197.  https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.2588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Graduate School of EducationHiroshima UniversityHigashihiroshimaJapan

Personalised recommendations