Skip to main content
Log in

Perception of self-handicapping behavior in the workplace: Not that great

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies that have examined the effectiveness of self-handicapping behavior as an impression management strategy have shown different results between students and workers. In order to generalize the results among workers, the present study examined the perception of self-handicapping behavior in the workplace by replicating and extending Park and Brown’s study (Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(2), 124–132, 2014) to workers in South Korea. To this end, 252 workers evaluated a target whose behavior (self-handicapping vs. control) and outcome (success vs. failure) were manipulated. Participants assessed the target in terms of attributions (to ability, effort, external factors, and luck) and their willingness to socialize and collaborate with the target. Results revealed that workers generally evaluated self-handicapping targets more negatively than control targets.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. An interaction between behavior and gender was also found, F(1, 211) = 4.78, p = .03, η2 = .02. Simple effects analyses revealed that when the target self-handicapped, women attributed both success and failure to effort more than men did, F(1, 211) = 7.12, p = .008, η2 = .03. When the target did not self-handicap, simple effects of gender were not significant, F(1, 211) = .24, p = .62. This is in line with previous research claiming that women value effort more than men (e.g., McCrea et al., 2008). However, such results do not pertain to our research topic of whether self-handicapping behavior is an effective impression management strategy; hence, there was no discussion of these findings.

  2. An interaction effect between behavior and gender was also significant, F(1, 211) = 6.65, p = .01, η2 = .03. Further analyses revealed a marginally significant simple effect of behavior among men such that men attributed both success and failure to external factors more in the self-handicapping condition than in the control condition, F(1, 211) = 3.75, p = .05, η2 = .02. A simple effect of behavior was not found among women, F(1, 211) = 2.92, p = .09. As mentioned in Footnote 1, these results were not discussed because they did not pertain to our hypotheses.

References

  • Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response to noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(4), 405–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, C. B., & Giuliano, T. A. (1999). Constructing obstacles vs. making excuses: Examining perceivers’ reactions to behavioral and self-reported self-handicapping. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 14(3), 419–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crant, J. M. (1996). Doing more harm than good: When is impression management likely to evoke a negative response? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(16), 1454–1471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crant, J. M., & Bateman, T. S. (1993). Assignment of credit and blame for performance outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 36(1), 7–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, J. R., & Patel, T. (2004). Social comparisons by procrastinators: Rating peers with similar or dissimilar delay tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(7), 1493–1501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, B. M., & Kernis, M. H. (2002). The role of authenticity in healthy psychological functioning and subjective well-being. Annals of the American Psychotherapy Association, 5(6), 18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (1996). “Forgive me, I’m new”: Three experimental demonstrations of the effects of attempts to excuse poor performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(2), 165–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., & McCrea, S. M. (2009). Man smart, woman smarter? Getting to the root of gender differences in self-handicapping. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(3), 260–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Deppe, R. K., & Gordon, L. J. (1991). Self-reported versus behavioral self-handicapping: Empirical evidence for a theoretical distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(6), 981–991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., McCrea, S. M., & Kimble, C. E. (2000). Public self-focus and sex differences in behavioral self-handicapping: Does increasing self-threat still make it “just a man’s game?”. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1131–1141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., McCrea, S. M., & Boris, H. I. (2003). “I know you self-handicapped last exam”: Gender differences in reactions to self-handicapping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 177–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, A. E., & Park, G. S. (2003). Nationalism, Confucianism, work ethic and industrialization in South Korea. Journal of Contemporary Asia, 33(1), 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, C. E., & Hirt, E. R. (2005). Self-focus, gender, and habitual self-handicapping: Do they make a difference in behavioral self-handicapping? Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 33(1), 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolditz, T. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1982). An impression management interpretation of the self-handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(3), 492–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., & Shepperd, J. A. (1986). Behavioral self-handicaps versus self-reported handicaps: A conceptual note. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1265–1268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luginbuhl, J., & Palmer, R. (1991). Impression management aspects of self-handicapping: Positive and negative effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(6), 655–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrea, S. M., & Hirt, E. R. (2001). The role of ability judgements in self-handicapping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1378–1389.

  • McCrea, S. M., Hirt, E. R., Hendrix, K. L., Milner, B. J., & Steele, N. L. (2008a). The worker scale: Developing a measure to explain gender differences in behavioral self-handicapping. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(4), 949–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrea, S. M., Hirt, E. R., & Milner, B. J. (2008b). She works hard for the money: Valuing effort underlies gender differences in behavioral self-handicapping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 292–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, C. B., & Warden, M. R. (1991). Implications of self-handicapping strategies for academic achievement: A reconceptualization. Journal of Social Psychology, 132(1), 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, S. W., & Brown, C. M. (2014). Different perceptions of self-handicapping across college and work contexts. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(2), 124–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. A. (2001). On the use of college students in social science research: Insights from a second-order meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(3), 450–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodewalt, F., Morf, C., Hazlett, S., & Fairfield, M. (1991). Self-handicapping: The role of discounting and augmentation in the preservation of self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 122–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepperd, J. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1989). Self-handicapping: The moderating roles of public self-consciousness and task importance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(2), 252–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Self-esteem, self-handicapping, and self-presentation: The strategy of inadequate practice. Journal of Personality, 58(2), 443–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1958). The protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. (T. Parsons Trans.). New York: Scribner. (Original work published 1905).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sun W. Park.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shin, H., Park, S.W. Perception of self-handicapping behavior in the workplace: Not that great. Curr Psychol 40, 910–918 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0013-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0013-6

Keywords

Navigation