Current Psychology

, Volume 35, Issue 3, pp 285–294 | Cite as

Adolescent Egocentrism and the Illusion of Transparency: Are Adolescents as Egocentric as we Might Think?

  • Roshan Rai
  • Peter Mitchell
  • Tasleem Kadar
  • Laura Mackenzie
Article
  • 831 Downloads

Abstract

The illusion of transparency, or people’s tendency to believe their thoughts and feelings as more apparent to others than they actually are, was used to investigate adolescent egocentrism. Contrary to previous research demonstrating heightened adolescent egocentrism, adolescents exhibited similar levels of egocentrism to adults. In experiment 1, 13-14 year-olds and adult participants both truthfully described and lied about a series of pictures. Both adolescent and adult liars indicated that they were more confident that other participants would know when they were lying, than other participants actually indicated. In experiment 2, 13-14 year-olds, 15-16 year-olds and adult participants read to an audience. The illusion of transparency effect manifested itself differently according to gender: Female participants indicating that they looked more nervous than audiences thought, whilst male participants indicating that they were more entertaining than audiences thought. Results were interpreted using simulation theory, and suggested that adolescents might not be as egocentric as previously thought. 

Keywords

Egocentrism Illusion of transparency Theory of mind Simulation theory Personal fable Imaginary audience Adolescence Adulthood  

References

  1. Aalsma, M. C., Lapsley, D. K., & Flannery, D. J. (2006). Personal fables, narcissism, and adolescent adjustment. Psychology in the Schools, 43(4), 481–491. doi:10.1002/pits.20162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alanazi, F. M. (2001). The revised self-consciousness scale: an assessment of factor structure, reliability, and gender differences in Saudi Arabia. Social Behavior and Personality, 29(8), 763–776. doi:10.2224/sbp.2001.29.8.763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alberts, A., Elkind, D., & Ginsberg, S. (2007). The personal fable and risk-taking in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 71–76. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9144-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Apperly, I. A. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825–839. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.676055.
  5. Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469–480. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469.
  6. Bell, J. H., & Bromnick, R. D. (2003). The social reality of the imaginary audience: a grounded thery approach. Adolescence, 38 (150).Google Scholar
  7. Cohn, L. D., Millstein, S. G., Irwin, C. E., Adler, N. E., Kegeles, S. M., Dolcini, P., & Stone, G. (1988). A comparison of two measure of egocentrism. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(2), 212–222. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5202_3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Dolcini, M. M., Cohn, L. D., Adler, N. E., Millstein, S. G., Irwin, C. E., Kegeles, S. M., & Stone, G. C. (1989). Adolescent egocentrism and feelings of invulnerability: are they related? The Journal of Early Adolescence, 9(4), 409–418. doi:10.1177/0272431689094003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025–1034. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1967.tb04378.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Elkind, D., & Bowen, R. (1979). Imaginary audience behavior in children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 15(1), 38–44. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.15.1.38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Elliott, G. R., & Feldman, S. S. (1990). Capturing the adolescent experience. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: the developing adolescent (pp. 1–14). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Endo, Y. (2007). Decision difficulty and illusion of transparency in Japanese university students. Psychological Reports, 100(2), 427–440. doi:10.2466/PR0.100.2.427-440.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Enright, R. D., Lapsley, D. K., & Shukla, D. G. (1979). Adolescent egocentrism in early and late adolescence. Adolescence, 14(56), 687–695.Google Scholar
  14. Enright, R. D., Shukla, D. G., & Lapsley, D. K. (1980). Adolescent egocentrism-sociocentrism and self-consciousness. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9(2), 101–116. doi:10.1007/BF02087929.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Frankenberger, K. D. (2000). Adolescent egocentrism: a comparison among adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 343–354. doi:10.1006/jado.2000.0319.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Galanaki, E. P. (2012). The imaginary audience and the personal fable: a test of Elkind’s theory of adolescent egocentrism. Psychology, 3(6), 457–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gilovich, T., & Savitsky, K. (1999). The spotlight effect and the illusion of transparency: egocentric assessments of how we are seen by others. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(6), 165–168. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (1998). The illusion of transparency: biased assessments of others’ ability to read one’s emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 332–346. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.332.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Goossens, L., Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Marcoen, A. (1992). The many faces of adolescent egocentrism: two European replications. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 43–58. doi:10.1177/074355489271004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goossens, L., Beyers, W., Emmen, M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2002). The imaginary audience and personal fable: factor analyses and concurrent validity of the “new look” measures. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12(2), 193–215. doi:10.1111/1532-7795.00031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gordon, R. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language, 1, 158–171. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1986.tb00324.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Greene, K., Rubin, D. L., Walters, L. H., & Hale, J. L. (1996). The utility of understanding adolescent egocentrism in designing health promotion messages. Health Communication, 8(2), 131–152. doi:10.1207/s15327027hc0802_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Greene, K., Krcmar, M., Rubin, D. L., Walters, L. H., & Hale, J. L. (2001). Elaboration in processing adolescent health messages: the impact of egocentrism and sensation seeking on message processing. Journal of Communication, 52(4), 812–831. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02575.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grenne, K., Rubin, D. L., & Hale, J. L. (1995). Egocentrism, message explicitness, and AIDS messages directed towards adolescents: an application of the theory of reasoned action. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 547–570.Google Scholar
  25. Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: the case for development. Mind and Language, 7, 120–144. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00201.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Holder, M. D., & Hawkins, C. (2007). The illusion of transparency: assessment of sex differences in showing and hiding disgust. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29(3), 235–243. doi:10.1080/01973530701503101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kruger, J., Epley, N., Parker, J., & Ng, Z. (2005). Egocentrism over email: can we communicate as well as we think? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 925–936. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.925.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Lapsley, D. K., FitzGerald, D. P., Rice, K. G., & Jackson, S. (1989). Separation-individuation and the “new look” at the imaginary audience and personal fable: a test of an integrative model. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 483–505. doi:10.1177/074355488944006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lempers, J. D., Flavell, E. R., & Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95(1), 3–53.Google Scholar
  30. Martin, J., & Sokol, B. (2011). Generalized others and imaginary audiences: a neo-Meadian approach to adolescent egocentrism. New Ideas in Psychology, 29(3), 364–375. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.03.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K. A., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., & Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual precepts of others. Child Development, 45, 357–366. doi:10.2307/1127956.
  32. Mitchell, P. (1996). Acquiring a conception of mind: A review of psychological research and theory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  33. Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what others know: imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 737–759. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Nickerson, R. S. (2001). The projective way of knowing: a useful heuristic that sometimes misleads. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(5), 168–172. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pullman, P. (1998). Northern lights. Southam, UK: Scholastic.Google Scholar
  36. Rai, R., & Mitchell, P. (2004). Five-year-old children’s difficulty with false belief when the sought entity is a person. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 112–126. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2004.05.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Rai, R., & Mitchell, P. (2006). Children’s ability to impute inferentially based knowledge. Child Development, 77(4), 1081–1093. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00920.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Rai, R., Mitchell, P., & Faelling, J. (2012). The illusion-of-transparency and episodic memory: are people egocentric or do people think lies are easy to detect? Psychological Studies, 57(1), 58–66. doi:10.1007/s12646-011-0138-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rankin, J. L., Lane, D. J., Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (2004). Adolescent self-consciousness: longitudinal age changes and gender differences in two cohorts. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14(1), 1–21. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2004.01401001.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Robins, R. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2005). Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 158–162. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tracy, J. L., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2002). Global self-esteem across the lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 17(3), 423–434. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.3.423.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Rosenroll, D. A. (1987). Early adolescent egocentrism: a review of six studies. Adolescence, 22(88), 791–802.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Rowling, J. K. (2000a). Harry Potter and the prisoner of Azkaban. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
  44. Rowling, J. K. (2000b). Harry Potter and the goblet of fire. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
  45. Savitsky, K., & Gilovich, T. (2003). The illusion of transparency and the alleviation of speech anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 618–625. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00056-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Savitsky, K., Elphy, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Do others judge us as harshly as we think? Overestimating the impact of our failures, shortcomings, and mishaps. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 44–56. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.44.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The self-consciousness scale: a revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15(8), 687–699. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1985.tb02268.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sodian, B., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Children’s understanding of inference as a source of knowledge. Child Development, 58, 424–433. doi:10.2307/1130519.
  49. Steinberg, L. (1999). Adolescence. Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  50. Swartz, P. D., Maynard, A. M., & Uzelac, S. M. (2008). Adolescent egocentrism: a contemporary view. Adolescence, 43(171), 441–448.Google Scholar
  51. Tolkein, J. R. R. (2001). The lord of the rings: The fellowship of the rings. London: Collins.Google Scholar
  52. Van Boven, L., Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (2003). The illusion of transparency in negotiations. Negotiation Journal, 19(2), 117–131. doi:10.1023/A:1023693700484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Vartanian, L. R. (2000). Revisiting the imaginary audience and personal fable constructs of adolescent egocentrism: a conceptual review. Adolescence, 35(140), 639–661.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Workman, J. E., & Lee, S. (2011). Vanity and public self-consciousness: a comparison of fashion consumer groups and gender. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 35(3), 307–325. doi:10.1111/j.1470-6431.2010.00934.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Yinon, Y., Mayraz, A., & Fox, S. (1994). Age and false-consensus effect. The Journal of Social Psychology, 134(6), 717–725. doi:10.1080/00224545.1994.9923006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Roshan Rai
    • 1
  • Peter Mitchell
    • 2
  • Tasleem Kadar
    • 1
  • Laura Mackenzie
    • 1
  1. 1.Division of PsychologyDe Montfort UniversityLeicesterUK
  2. 2.University of Nottingham Malaysia CampusSemenyihMalaysia

Personalised recommendations