Current Psychology

, Volume 21, Issue 4, pp 293–318 | Cite as

We think you agree: The detrimental impact of the false consensus effect on behavior

  • Kathleen P. Bauman
  • Glenn Geher
Article

Abstract

Two studies were designed to examine the impact of the false consensus effect on behavior (FCE; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). False consensus is a form of social projection whereby individuals overestimate the degree to which others share their characteristics or beliefs. In a modified test of the theory of reasoned action, Study 1 demonstrated that the FCE independently predicts behavioral intentions regarding important social issues in a sample comprised of 205 college students. Further, results indicated that self-monitoring moderates the extent to which the FCE predicts behavioral intentions (specifically, as hypothesized, the FCE is a stronger predictor of behavior for high self-monitors). Because of the prevalence of the FCE among college students regarding potentially harmful social behaviors, Study 2 was designed to eliminate the FCE by differentially presenting students (N = 280) with alternative viewpoints regarding various issues. Presenting both sides of an argument using video-based stimuli effectively reduced the FCE. Recommendations for interventions that effectively promote beneficial social norms are discussed.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Asch, S.E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193, 31–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago, IL: Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
  3. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Botvin, G.J., Botvin, E.M., Baker, E., Dusenbury, L., & Goldberg, C.J. (1992). The false consensus effect: Predicting adolescents' tobacco use from normative expectations. Psychological Reports, 70, 171–178.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chan, D.K., & Fishbein, M. (1993). Determinants of college women's intentions to tell their partners to use condoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1455–1470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chassin, L., Presson, C.C., Sherman, S.J., Corty, E., & Olshavsky, R.W. (1984). Predicting the onset of cigarette smoking in adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 224–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Crowne, D.P. & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Deutsch, F. (1987). When friends lead us astray: Evidence for the selective exposure hypothesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 128 (2), 271–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dielman, T.E., Campanelli, P.C., Shope, J.T., & Butchart, A.T. (1987). Susceptibility to peer pressure, self-esteem, and health locus of control as correlates of adolescent substance abuse. Health Education Quarterly, 14, 207–221.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fishbein, M. (1979). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. In H. Howe and M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of New Hampshire Press.Google Scholar
  13. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  14. Funder, D.C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1974). Short forms of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI): An objective measure of self-esteem. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 473–475.Google Scholar
  16. Holmes, D.S. (1968). Dimensions of projection. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 248–268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kelly, J.A., Lawrence, J.S., Diaz, Y.E., Stevenson, L.Y., Hauth, A.C., Brasfield, T.L., Kalichman, S.C., Smith, J.E., & Andrew, M.E. (1991). HIV risk behavior reduction following intervention with key opinion leaders of population: An experimental analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 81, 168–171.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kelly, J.A., Lawrence, J.S., Stevenson, L.Y., Hauth, A.C., Kalichman, S.C., Diaz, Y.E., Brasfield, T.L., Koob, J.J., & Morgan, M.G. (1992). Community AIDS/HIV risk reduction: The effects of endorsements by popular people in three cities. American Journal of Public Health, 82, 1483–1489.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Kraus, S.J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 58–75.Google Scholar
  20. Krueger, J., & Clement, R.W. (1994). The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 596–610.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Krueger, J., & Zeiger, J.S. (1993). Social categorization and the truly false consensus effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 670–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Laflin, M.T., Moore-Hirschl, S. Weis, D.L, & Hayes, B.E. (1994). Use of the theory of reasoned action to predict drug and alcohol use. The International Journal of the Addictions, 29(7), 927–940.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Marks, G., & Duval, S. (1991). Availability of alternative positions and estimates of consensus. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 179–183.Google Scholar
  24. Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 72–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Miller, N., & Marks, G. (1982). Assumed similarity between self and other: Effect of expectation of future interaction with that other. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 100–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Morrison, D.M., Gillmore, M.R., & Baker, S.A. (1995). Determinants of condom use among high-risk heterosexual adults: A test of the theory of reasoned action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 651–676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mullen, B., Atkins, J.L., Champion, D.S., Edwards, C., Hardy, D., Story, J.E., & Vanderklok, M. (1985). The false consensus effect: A meta-analysis of 115 hypothesis tests. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 262–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Newcomb, T.M. (1943). Personality and social change. New York: Dryden.Google Scholar
  29. Norman, N.M., & Tedeschi, J.T. (1989). Self-presentation, reasoned action, and adolescents' decisions to smoke cigarettes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19(1), 543–558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Prentice, D.A., & Miller, D.T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 243–256.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Prislin, R., & Kovrlija, N. (1992). Predicting behavior of high and low self-monitors: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Psychological Reports, 70, 1131–1138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ross, M.W., & McLaws, M.L. (1992). Subjective norms about condoms are better predictors of use and intention to use than attitudes. Health Education Research, 7(3), 335–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sharp, M.J., & Getz, J.G. (1996). Substance use as impression management. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(1), 60–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  36. Sherman, S.J., Presson, C.C., Chassin, L., Corty, E., & Olshavsky, R. (1983). The false consensus effect in estimates of smoking prevalence: Underlying mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 197–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Suls, J., & Wills, T. (Eds.). (1991). Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Tashakkori, A., & Thompson, V. D. (1992). Predictors of intention to take precautions against AIDS among black college students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 736–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Trafimow, D. (1994). Predicting intentions to use a condom from perceptions of normative pressure and confidence in those perceptions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2151–2163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. van den Putte, B. (1991). Twenty years of the theory of reasoned action of Fishbein and Ajzen: A meta-analysis. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  43. White, K.M., Terry, D.J., & Hogg, M.A. (1994). Safer sex behavior: The role of attitudes, norms, and control factors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2164–2192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kathleen P. Bauman
    • 1
  • Glenn Geher
    • 2
  1. 1.University of MaineUSA
  2. 2.State University of New YorkNew Paltz

Personalised recommendations