Skip to main content
Log in

We think you agree: The detrimental impact of the false consensus effect on behavior

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies were designed to examine the impact of the false consensus effect on behavior (FCE; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). False consensus is a form of social projection whereby individuals overestimate the degree to which others share their characteristics or beliefs. In a modified test of the theory of reasoned action, Study 1 demonstrated that the FCE independently predicts behavioral intentions regarding important social issues in a sample comprised of 205 college students. Further, results indicated that self-monitoring moderates the extent to which the FCE predicts behavioral intentions (specifically, as hypothesized, the FCE is a stronger predictor of behavior for high self-monitors). Because of the prevalence of the FCE among college students regarding potentially harmful social behaviors, Study 2 was designed to eliminate the FCE by differentially presenting students (N = 280) with alternative viewpoints regarding various issues. Presenting both sides of an argument using video-based stimuli effectively reduced the FCE. Recommendations for interventions that effectively promote beneficial social norms are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asch, S.E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193, 31–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago, IL: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botvin, G.J., Botvin, E.M., Baker, E., Dusenbury, L., & Goldberg, C.J. (1992). The false consensus effect: Predicting adolescents' tobacco use from normative expectations. Psychological Reports, 70, 171–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, D.K., & Fishbein, M. (1993). Determinants of college women's intentions to tell their partners to use condoms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1455–1470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chassin, L., Presson, C.C., Sherman, S.J., Corty, E., & Olshavsky, R.W. (1984). Predicting the onset of cigarette smoking in adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 224–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D.P. & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, F. (1987). When friends lead us astray: Evidence for the selective exposure hypothesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 128 (2), 271–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dielman, T.E., Campanelli, P.C., Shope, J.T., & Butchart, A.T. (1987). Susceptibility to peer pressure, self-esteem, and health locus of control as correlates of adolescent substance abuse. Health Education Quarterly, 14, 207–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M. (1979). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. In H. Howe and M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of New Hampshire Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D.C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1974). Short forms of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI): An objective measure of self-esteem. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 473–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, D.S. (1968). Dimensions of projection. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 248–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J.A., Lawrence, J.S., Diaz, Y.E., Stevenson, L.Y., Hauth, A.C., Brasfield, T.L., Kalichman, S.C., Smith, J.E., & Andrew, M.E. (1991). HIV risk behavior reduction following intervention with key opinion leaders of population: An experimental analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 81, 168–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J.A., Lawrence, J.S., Stevenson, L.Y., Hauth, A.C., Kalichman, S.C., Diaz, Y.E., Brasfield, T.L., Koob, J.J., & Morgan, M.G. (1992). Community AIDS/HIV risk reduction: The effects of endorsements by popular people in three cities. American Journal of Public Health, 82, 1483–1489.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus, S.J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 58–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Clement, R.W. (1994). The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 596–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Zeiger, J.S. (1993). Social categorization and the truly false consensus effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 670–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laflin, M.T., Moore-Hirschl, S. Weis, D.L, & Hayes, B.E. (1994). Use of the theory of reasoned action to predict drug and alcohol use. The International Journal of the Addictions, 29(7), 927–940.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, G., & Duval, S. (1991). Availability of alternative positions and estimates of consensus. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 179–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 72–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N., & Marks, G. (1982). Assumed similarity between self and other: Effect of expectation of future interaction with that other. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 100–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, D.M., Gillmore, M.R., & Baker, S.A. (1995). Determinants of condom use among high-risk heterosexual adults: A test of the theory of reasoned action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 651–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, B., Atkins, J.L., Champion, D.S., Edwards, C., Hardy, D., Story, J.E., & Vanderklok, M. (1985). The false consensus effect: A meta-analysis of 115 hypothesis tests. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 262–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, T.M. (1943). Personality and social change. New York: Dryden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, N.M., & Tedeschi, J.T. (1989). Self-presentation, reasoned action, and adolescents' decisions to smoke cigarettes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19(1), 543–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D.A., & Miller, D.T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 243–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prislin, R., & Kovrlija, N. (1992). Predicting behavior of high and low self-monitors: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Psychological Reports, 70, 1131–1138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M.W., & McLaws, M.L. (1992). Subjective norms about condoms are better predictors of use and intention to use than attitudes. Health Education Research, 7(3), 335–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, M.J., & Getz, J.G. (1996). Substance use as impression management. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(1), 60–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, S.J., Presson, C.C., Chassin, L., Corty, E., & Olshavsky, R. (1983). The false consensus effect in estimates of smoking prevalence: Underlying mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 197–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., & Wills, T. (Eds.). (1991). Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tashakkori, A., & Thompson, V. D. (1992). Predictors of intention to take precautions against AIDS among black college students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 736–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, D. (1994). Predicting intentions to use a condom from perceptions of normative pressure and confidence in those perceptions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2151–2163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Putte, B. (1991). Twenty years of the theory of reasoned action of Fishbein and Ajzen: A meta-analysis. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, K.M., Terry, D.J., & Hogg, M.A. (1994). Safer sex behavior: The role of attitudes, norms, and control factors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 2164–2192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bauman, K.P., Geher, G. We think you agree: The detrimental impact of the false consensus effect on behavior. Curr Psychol 21, 293–318 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-002-1020-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-002-1020-0

Keywords

Navigation