Advertisement

Human-Computer Interaction Problem in Learning: Could the Key Be Hidden Somewhere Between Social Interaction and Development of Tools?

  • Tolga YıldızEmail author
Regular Article

Abstract

Homo sapiens is not just a tool-using species, they also can invent and develop tools. This is the feature that distinguishes humans from other species. It is necessary to get rid of the perceptual dominance of the present state of the material to invent and develop a tool. It is based on a mental process: designing. So how was that possible? In this article, I propose an evolutionary hypothesis in response to this question: the referential triangle. Accordingly, the relationship that people establish with things is mentally indirect, but the relationship they establish with each other is mentally direct. The hypothesis claims that the mental solutions of people have naturally established with each other are also used to invent and develop tools. The latest and most interesting product of this mechanism is artificial intelligence. Because artificial intelligence also acts as an inorganic system. What distinguishes it from other machines in this context is its social behavior. Artificial intelligence can generate social signals. So can artificial intelligence be both a tool and a partner at the same time in the referential triangle established with the tools by the human? In other words, can children, for instance, socially interact with artificial intelligence, just as they do naturally with people around? The article draws attention to that this problem should be included in the cultural psychological research agenda.

Keywords

Social interaction Human-computer interaction Referential triangle Cultural learning Human evolution Artificial intelligence 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by the author.

References

  1. Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. A. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72(2), 416–430.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bacher, K., Allen, S., Lindholm, A. K., Bejder, L., & Krützen, M. (2010). Genes or culture: are mitochondrial genes associated with tool use in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.)? Behavior Genetics, 40, 706–714.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination (M. Holquist, Ed., C. Emerson, Trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays (C. Emerson, M. Holquist, Eds. & V. W. Mc Gee, Trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  5. Balaban, M. T., & Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 3–26.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Barnard, A. (2014). Simgesel Düşüncenin Doğuşu [Genesis of Symbolic Thought] (M. Doğan, Trans.). İstanbul: Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Yayınları.Google Scholar
  7. Barnard, C., et al. (Producers) & Jonze, S. (Director) (2013). Her [Motion Picture]. USA: Annapurna Pictures.Google Scholar
  8. Barsalou, L. W. (2012). The human conceptual system. In M. Spivey, K. McRae, & M. Joanisse (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 239–258). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of times east: absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1635–1639.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2016). Navigating pedagogy: children’s developing capacities for learning from pedagogical interactions. Cognitive Development, 38, 27–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2010). Language and the development of cognitive control. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 631–642.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes, F. G., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330, 1359–1364.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Ferguson, B., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Communicative signals support abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants. Scientific Reports, 6, 25434.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Developmental Review, 28(2), 225–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hasse, C. (2015). An anthropology of learning. New York/London: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., & Keysers, C. (2012). Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 114–121.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hirsh-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Gray, J. H., Robb, M. B., & Kaufman, J. (2015). Putting education in “educational” apps: lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16(1), 3–34.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge an imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. J. (2011). Evrimin Dört Boyutu: Yaşam Tarihinde Genetik, Epigenetik, Davranışsal ve Simgesel Değişimler [Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life] (M. Doğan, Trans.). İstanbul: Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Yayınları.Google Scholar
  24. Kubrick, S. (Producer & Director) (1968). 2001: A Space Odyssey [Motion Picture]. UK, USA: Metro-Goldwyn-Mayer (MGM), Stanley Kubrick Productions.Google Scholar
  25. Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(15), 9096–9101.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Lewkowicz, D. J., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2009). The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 470–478.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Lewontin, R. C., Rose, S., & Kamin, L. J. (2018). Genlerimizden İbaret Değiliz: Biyoloji, İdeoloji ve İnsan Doğası [Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature] (G. K. Gevinç et al., Trans.). İstanbul: Yordam. (Original work published 1984).Google Scholar
  28. Liu, X., Somel, M., Tang, L., Yan, Z., Jiang, X., Guo, S., Yuan, Y., He, L., Oleksiak, A., Zhang, Y., Li, N., Hu, Y., Chen, W., Qiu, Z., Pääbo, S., & Khaitovich, P. (2012). Extension of cortical synaptic development distinguishes humans from chimpanzees and macaques. Genome Research, 22(4), 611–622.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Liu, Y., Piazza, E. A., Simony, E., Shewokis, P. A., Onaral, B., Hasson, U., & Ayaz, H. (2017). Measuring speaker-listener neural coupling with functional near infrared spectroscopy. Scientific Reports, 7, 43293.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Luria, A. R., & Vygotsky, L. S. (1992). Ape, primitive man, and child: Essays in the history of behaviour. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf (Original work published 1930).Google Scholar
  31. Nazzi, T., & Gopnik, A. (2001). Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: when does language become a tool for categorization? Cognition, 80, B11–B20.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Okumura, Y., Kanakogi, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Itakura, S. (2013). The power of human gaze on infant learning. Cognition, 128, 127–133.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Osiurak, F., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2018). How our cognition shapes and is shaped by technology: a common framework for understanding human tool-use interactions in the past, present, and future. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 293.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Pan, Y. (2016). Heading toward artificial intelligence 2.0. Engineering, 2, 409–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S. (2004). Do both pictures and words function as symbols for 18- and 24-month-old children? Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 185–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(179), i.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85(3), 956–970.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Searle, J. R. (2006). Zihin Dil Toplum, Gerçek Dünyada Felsefe [Mind, Language And Society: Philosophy In The Real World] (A. Tural, Trans.). İstanbul: Litera Yayıncılık. (Original work published 1998).Google Scholar
  40. Sennett, R. (2012). Beraber [Together] (İ. Özküralpli, Trans.). İstanbul: Ayrıntı Yayınları.Google Scholar
  41. Şerif, M. (1985). Toplumsal Kuralların Psikolojisi [The Psychology of Social Norms] (İ. Sandıkçıoğlu, Trans.). İstanbul: Alan Yayıncılık. (Original work published 1936).Google Scholar
  42. Stephens, G. J., Silbert, L. J., & Hasson, U. (2010). Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(32), 14425–14430.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Strouse, G. A., & Ganea, P. A. (2017). Toddlers’ word learning and transfer from electronic and print books. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 129–142.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Strouse, G. A., & Troseth, G. L. (2014). Supporting toddlers’ transfer of word learning from video. Cognitive Development, 30, 47–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sullivan, J., & Barner, D. (2016). Discourse bootstrapping: preschoolers use linguistic discourse to learn new words. Developmental Science, 19(1), 63–75.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate? Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. London: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 675–735.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 56, 1–20.Google Scholar
  54. Turing, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 49, 433–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Valsiner, J. (2014). An invitation to cultural psychology. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum. (Original work published 1934).Google Scholar
  58. Warneken, F. (2013). Young children proactively remedy unnoticed accidents. Cognition, 126, 101–108.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Yıldız, T. (2013). Bir kelime neyi değiştirir? Boyut değiştirerek eşleme görevine kavramsal ağların etkisi [What does a word alter? The effect of conceptual networks on the dimensional change sorting task]. Nesne Psikoloji Dergisi, 1(1), 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Zack, E., & Barr, R. (2016). The role of interactional quality in learning from touch screens during infancy: context matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIstanbul UniversityIstanbulTurkey

Personalised recommendations