Advertisement

Human Nature

, Volume 30, Issue 1, pp 23–58 | Cite as

Evolutionary Models of Leadership

Tests and Synthesis
  • Zachary H. Garfield
  • Robert L. Hubbard
  • Edward H. HagenEmail author
Article

Abstract

This study tested four theoretical models of leadership with data from the ethnographic record. The first was a game-theoretical model of leadership in collective actions, in which followers prefer and reward a leader who monitors and sanctions free-riders as group size increases. The second was the dominance model, in which dominant leaders threaten followers with physical or social harm. The third, the prestige model, suggests leaders with valued skills and expertise are chosen by followers who strive to emulate them. The fourth proposes that in small-scale, kin-based societies, men with high neural capital are best able to achieve and maintain positions of social influence (e.g., as headmen) and thereby often become polygynous and have more offspring than other men, which positively selects for greater neural capital. Using multiple search strategies we identified more than 1000 texts relevant to leadership in the Probability Sample of 60 cultures from the Human Relations Area Files (HRAF). We operationalized the model with variables and then coded all retrieved text records on the presence or absence of evidence for each of these 24 variables. We found mixed support for the collective action model, broad support for components of the prestige leadership style and the importance of neural capital and polygyny among leaders, but more limited support for the dominance leadership style. We found little evidence, however, of emulation of, or prestige-biased learning toward, leaders. We found that improving collective actions, having expertise, providing counsel, and being respected, having high neural capital, and being polygynous are common properties of leaders, which warrants a synthesis of the collective action, prestige, and neural capital and reproductive skew models. We sketch one such synthesis involving high-quality decision-making and other computational services.

Keywords

Leadership Prestige Dominance Collective action Neural capital Cross-cultural 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Barry Hewlett, Raymond Hames, and Leslie New for helpful comments during the preparation of this manuscript. Additionally, this manuscript was greatly improved thanks to thorough review by Mark Van Vugt, Chris von Rueden, and an anonymous reviewer. This research was funded by National Science Foundation Division of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences award 1628509. Original submission: August 16, 2017; Acceptance: May 17, 2018

Supplementary material

12110_2019_9338_MOESM1_ESM.docx (38 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 38.1 kb)
12110_2019_9338_MOESM2_ESM.xls (106 kb)
ESM 2 (XLS 106 kb)

References

  1. Adriani, N., & Kruijt, A. C. (1950). The Bare’e-speaking Toradja of central Celebes (the East Toradja), vol. 1. [De Bare’e sprekende Toradjas van Midden-Celebes (de Oost-Toradjas), eerste deel, Amsterdam: Noord-Hollandsche Uitgevers Maatschappij]. (HRAF MS: 13, 714 [original: 12, 478]). http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=og11-002
  2. Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan Special Publication, 1. Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
  3. Apter, D. E. (1967). Political kingdom in Uganda: A study in bureaucratic nationalism. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Arden, R., Gottfredson, L. S., & Miller, G. (2009). Does a fitness factor contribute to the association between intelligence and health outcomes? Evidence from medical abnormality counts among 3654 US veterans. Intelligence, 37(6), 581–591.Google Scholar
  5. Arnold, J. E. (1996). Organizational transformations: Power and labor among complex hunter-gatherers and other intermediate societies. In J. E. Arnold (Ed.), Emergent complexity: The evolution of intermediate societies (pp. 59–73). Ann Arbor: International Monographs in Prehistory.Google Scholar
  6. Barclay, P. (2011). Competitive helping increases with the size of biological markets and invades defection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 281(1), 47–55.Google Scholar
  7. Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
  8. Barkow, J. H., Akiwowo, A. A., Barua, T. K., Chance, M. R., Chapple, E. D., Chattopadhyay, G. P., et al. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology, 16(4), 553–572.Google Scholar
  9. Bass, B. M., & Stogdill, R. M. (1990). Handbook of leadership: Theory, research & managerial applications (third ed.). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  10. Birdsell, J. B. (1958). On population structure in generalized hunting and collecting populations. Evolution, 12(2), 189–205.Google Scholar
  11. Blader, S. L., & Chen, Y.-R. (2014). What’s in a name? Status, power, and other forms of social hierarchy. In J. T. Cheng, J. L. Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.), The psychology of social status (pp. 71–95). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  12. Boehm, C. (1993). Egalitarian behavior and reverse dominance hierarchy. Current Anthropology, 34(3), 227–254.Google Scholar
  13. Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Boehm, C. (2008). Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism. Cross-Cultural Research, 42(4), 319–352.Google Scholar
  15. Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Fazzio, I., Irons, W., McElreath, R. L., Bowles, S., Bell, A., Hertz, T., & Hazzah, L. (2010). Pastoralism and wealth inequality: Revisiting an old question. Current Anthropology, 51(1), 35–48.Google Scholar
  16. Bowser, B., & Patton, J. (2010). Women’s leadership: Political alliance, economic resources, and reproductive success in the Ecuadorian Amazon. In K. J. Vaughn, J. W. Eerkens, & J. Kantner (Eds.), The evolution of leadership: Transitions in decision making from small-scale to middle-range societies (pp. 51–71). Santa Fe: SAR Press.Google Scholar
  17. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171–195.Google Scholar
  18. Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  19. Byrne, R., & Whiten, A. (1989). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Stewart, M., & Manning, J. G. (2002). The formation of status hierarchies in leaderless groups. Human Nature, 13(3), 345–362.Google Scholar
  21. Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science, 239(4843), 985–992.Google Scholar
  22. Chance, M. R. (1967). Attention structure as the basis of primate rank orders. Man, 2(4), 503–518.Google Scholar
  23. Chapais, B. (2015). Competence and the evolutionary origins of status and power in humans. Human Nature, 26(2), 161–183.Google Scholar
  24. Chaudhary, N., Salali, G. D., Thompson, J., Dyble, M., Page, A., Smith, D., Mace, R., & Migliano, A. B. (2015). Polygyny without wealth: Popularity in gift games predicts polygyny in BaYaka pygmies. Royal Society Open Science, 2(5), 150054.Google Scholar
  25. Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 334–347.Google Scholar
  26. Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(1), 103–125.Google Scholar
  27. Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Anderson, C. (Eds.). (2014). The psychology of social status. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  28. Cohen, R., & Middleton, J. (Eds.). (1967). Comparative political systems: Studies in the politics of pre-industrial societies. Garden City: The Natural History Press.Google Scholar
  29. Cramb, R. A. (1986). Evolution of Iban land tenure. Working Papers. Clayton, Australia: Monash University, Centre of Southeast Asian Studies. http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=oc06-023
  30. Drews, C. (1993). The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour. Behaviour, 125(3), 283–313.Google Scholar
  31. Dyble, M., Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Page, A., Smith, D., Thompson, J., Vinicius, L., Mace, R., & Migliano, A. B. (2015). Sex equality can explain the unique social structure of hunter-gatherer bands. Science, 348(6236), 796–798.Google Scholar
  32. Dyer, J. R. G., Johansson, A., Helbing, D., Couzin, I. D., & Krause, J. (2009). Leadership, consensus decision making and collective behaviour in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1518), 781–789.Google Scholar
  33. Earle, T. K. (1997). How chiefs come to power: The political economy in prehistory. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1928). Dance. Africa (Vol. 1, pp. 446–462). London: Oxford University Press http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=fo07-006.Google Scholar
  35. Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980–994.Google Scholar
  36. Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1), 10–46.Google Scholar
  37. Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 1–27.Google Scholar
  38. Frank, S. A. (2003). Perspective: Repression of competition and the evolution of cooperation. Evolution, 57(4), 693–705.Google Scholar
  39. Freeman, D. (1981). Some reflections on the nature of Iban society. Canberra, ACT: Department of Anthropology, Research School of Pacific Studies, Australian National University.Google Scholar
  40. Fried, M. H. (1967). The evolution of political society. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  41. Friedman, J., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2010). Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical Software, 33(1), 1–22.Google Scholar
  42. Garfield, Z. H., Garfield, M. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2016). A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. In B. S. Hewlett & T. Hideaki (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 19–34). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  43. Gavrilets, S., & Fortunato, L. (2014). A solution to the collective action problem in between-group conflict with within-group inequality. Nature Communications, 5, 3526.Google Scholar
  44. Gavrilets, S., Auerbach, J., & Van Vugt, M. (2016). Convergence to consensus in heterogeneous groups and the emergence of informal leadership. Scientific Reports, 6, 29704.Google Scholar
  45. Glowacki, L., & von Rueden, C. R. (2015). Leadership solves collective action problems in small-scale societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1683), 20150010.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0010.Google Scholar
  46. Glowacki, L., & Wrangham, R. W. (2013). The role of rewards in motivating participation in simple warfare. Human Nature, 24(4), 444–460.Google Scholar
  47. Grammer, K. (1996). Symptoms of power: The function and evolution of social status. In P. Wiessner & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), Food and the status quest: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 68–85). Providence: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
  48. Gray, J. P. (2003). World cultures. Journal of Comparative and Cross-Cultural Research, 14(1).Google Scholar
  49. Guala, F. (2012). Reciprocity: Weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 1–15.Google Scholar
  50. Gurven, M., & von Rueden, C. R. (2006). Hunting, social status and biological fitness. Biodemography and Social Biology, 53, 81–99.Google Scholar
  51. Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, M. (2000). “It’s a wonderful life”: Signaling generosity among the ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(4), 263–282.Google Scholar
  52. Gusinde, M., & Schütze, F. (1937). Yahgan: The life and thought of the water nomads of Cape Horn. Die Feuerland-Indianer [The Fuegian Indians]. Mödling Bei Wein: Anthropos-Bibliothek. http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=sh06-001
  53. Gwet, K. L. (2014). Handbook of inter-rater reliability: The definitive guide to measuring the extent of agreement among raters. Gaithersburg: Advanced Analytics, LLC.Google Scholar
  54. Hagen, E. H., Chater, N., Gallistel, C. R., Houston, A., Kacelnik, A., Kalenscher, T., et al. (2012). Decision making: What can evolution do for us? In P. Hammerstein & J. R. Stevens (Eds.), Evolution and the mechanisms of decision making: Toward a Darwinian decision theory (pp. 97–126). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  55. Hames, R. (2015). Kin selection. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 505–523). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  56. Hamstra, M. R. (2014). Big men: Male leaders’ height positively relates to followers’ perception of charisma. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 190–192.Google Scholar
  57. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196.Google Scholar
  58. Henrich, N., & Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277(1701), 3715–3724.Google Scholar
  60. Henrich, J., Chudek, M., & Boyd, R. (2015). The big man mechanism: How prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 370(1683), 20150013.Google Scholar
  61. Hess, N. H., & Hagen, E. H. (2006). Sex differences in indirect aggression: Psychological evidence from young adults. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(3), 231–245.Google Scholar
  62. Hess, N. H., & Hagen, E. H. (2017). Gossip, reputation, and friendship in in-group competition: An evolutionary perspective. In F. Giardini & R. Wittek (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of gossip and reputation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Demography and childcare in preindustrial societies. Journal of Anthropological Research, 47, 1–37.Google Scholar
  64. Hoebel, E. A. (1954). The law of primitive man: A study in comparative legal dynamics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  65. Hollis, A. C., with an introduction by S. C. Eliot. (1905). Masai: Their language and folklore. Oxford: Clarendon Press. http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=fl12-001.
  66. Hooper, P. L., Kaplan, H. S., & Boone, J. L. (2010). A theory of leadership in human cooperative groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 265(4), 633–646.Google Scholar
  67. Johnson, A. W., & Earle, T. K. (1987). The evolution of human societies: From foraging group to agrarian state. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Johnstone, R. A. (2000). Models of reproductive skew: A review and synthesis. Ethology, 106, 5–26.Google Scholar
  69. Judge, T. A., Colbert, A. E., & Ilies, R. (2004). Intelligence and leadership: A quantitative review and test of theoretical propositions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 542–552.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.542.Google Scholar
  70. Kantner, J. (2010). Identifying the pathways to permanent leadership. In K. J. Vaughn, J. W. Eerkens, & J. Kantner (Eds.), The evolution of leadership: Transitions in decision making from small-scale to middle-range societies (pp. 249–281). Santa Fe: SAR Press.Google Scholar
  71. Kaplan, H. S., Lancaster, J. B., Johnson, S. E., & Bock, J. A. (1995). Does observed fertility maximize fitness among New Mexican men? Human Nature, 6(4), 325–360.Google Scholar
  72. Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4), 156–185.Google Scholar
  73. Kaplan, H. S., Mueller, T., Gangestad, S., & Lancaster, J. B. (2003a). Neural capital and life span evolution among primates and humans. In C. E. Finch, J. M. Robine, & Y. Christen (Eds.), Brain and longevity (pp. 69–97). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  74. Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J., & Robson, A. (2003b). Embodied capital and the evolutionary economics of the human life span. Population and Development Review, 29, 152–182.Google Scholar
  75. Kaplan, H. S., Hooper, P. L., & Gurven, M. (2009). The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3289–3299.Google Scholar
  76. Keohane, N. O. (2010). Thinking about leadership. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  77. King, A. J., Johnson, D. D. P., & Van Vugt, M. (2009). The origins and evolution of leadership. Current Biology, 19(19), PR911–916.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.027, R911.
  78. Kiyonari, T., & Barclay, P. (2008). Cooperation in social dilemmas: Free riding may be thwarted by second-order reward rather than by punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 826–842.Google Scholar
  79. Kohler, T. A., Cockburn, D., Hooper, P. L., Bocinsky, R. K., & Kobti, Z. (2012). The coelvoution of group size and leadership: An agent-based public goods model for prehispanic Pueblo societies. Advances in Complex Systems, 15(1, n. 2), 1150007.Google Scholar
  80. Kracke, W. H. (1978). Force and persuasion. Leadership in an Amazonian society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  81. Kurzban, R., Burton-Chellew, M. N., & West, S. A. (2015). The evolution of altruism in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 575–599.Google Scholar
  82. Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211–240.Google Scholar
  83. Laustsen, L., & Petersen, M. B. (2017). Perceived conflict and leader dominance: Individual and contextual factors behind preferences for dominant leaders. Political Psychology, 38(6), 1083–1101.Google Scholar
  84. Lebel, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2011). Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 10937–10947.Google Scholar
  85. Lewis, H. S. (1974). Leaders and followers: Some anthropological perspectives. Boston: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  86. Lindee, S. (2001). James Van Gundia Neel (1915–2000). American Anthropologist, 103(2), 502–505.Google Scholar
  87. Linton, R. (1936). The study of man: An introduction. New York and London: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  88. Low, B. S. (2003). Ecological and social complexities in human monogamy. In U. H. Reichard & C. Boesch (Eds.), Monogamy: Mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans and other mammals (pp. 161–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Low, B. S. (2005). Women’s lives there, here, then, now: A review of women’s ecological and demographic constraints cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 64–87.Google Scholar
  90. Lukaszewski, A. W., Simmons, Z. L., Anderson, C., & Roney, J. R. (2016). The role of physical formidability in human social status allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 385–406.Google Scholar
  91. Macfarlan, S. J., Remiker, M., & Quinlan, R. (2012). Competitive altruism explains labor exchange variation in a Dominican community. Current Anthropology, 53, 118–124.Google Scholar
  92. Marshall, L. (1960). Kung Bushman bands. Africa, 30(04), 325–355.Google Scholar
  93. Mattison, S. M., Smith, E. A., Shenk, M. K., & Cochrane, E. E. (2016). The evolution of inequality. Evolutionary Anthropology, 25, 184–199.Google Scholar
  94. Maynard Smith, J., & Parker, G. A. (1976). The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal Behaviour, 24(1), 159–175.Google Scholar
  95. Mead, M. (1935). Sex and temperament in three primitive societies. London: G. Routledge & Sons.Google Scholar
  96. Miller, G. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture: An interdisciplinary view (pp. 71–91). New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
  97. Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
  98. Moïse, R. (2014). “Do pygmies have a history?” revisited: The autochthonous tradition in the history of Equatorial Africa. In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.) Hunter-gatherers of the Congo Basin: Cultures, histories and biology of African Pygmies (pp. 85–116). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  99. Moore, M. (1975). Co-operative labour in peasant agriculture. Journal of Peasant Studies, 2(3), 270–291.Google Scholar
  100. Munoz, S. R., & Bangdiwala, S. I. (1997). Interpretation of Kappa and B statistics measures of agreement. Journal of Applied Statistics, 24(1), 105–112.Google Scholar
  101. Murdock, G. P. (1949). Social structure. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  102. Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Measurement of cultural complexity. Ethnology, 12(4), 379–392.Google Scholar
  103. Murdock, G. P., & White, D. R. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 8(4), 329–369.Google Scholar
  104. Murdock, G. P., Ford, C. S., Hudson, A. E., Kennedy, R., Simmons, L. W., & Whiting, J. W. (2008). Outline of cultural materials (6th ed.). New Haven: Human Relations Area Files.Google Scholar
  105. Næss, M. W., Bårdsen, B.-J., Fauchald, P., & Tveraa, T. (2010). Cooperative pastoral production–the importance of kinship. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(4), 246–258.Google Scholar
  106. Naroll, R. (1967). The proposed HRAF probability sample. Cross-Cultural Research, 2(2), 70–80.Google Scholar
  107. Neel, J. V. (1970). Lessons from a “primitive” people. Science, 170(960), 815–822.Google Scholar
  108. Neel, J. V. (1980). On being headman. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 23, 277–294.Google Scholar
  109. Neel, J. V., & Salzano, F. M. (1967). Further studies on the Xavante Indians, X: Some hypotheses-generalizations resulting from these studies. American Journal of Human Genetics, 19(4), 554–574.Google Scholar
  110. Neel, J. V., & Sang, J. H. (1994). Physician to the gene pool. Genetical Research, 64(2), 145–148.Google Scholar
  111. Neel, J. V., Salzano, F. M., Junqueira, P. C., Keiter, F., & Maybury-Lewis, D. (1964). Studies on the Xavante Indians of the Brazilian Mato Grosso. American Journal of Human Genetics, 16, 52–140.Google Scholar
  112. Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), 1560–1563.Google Scholar
  113. O’Gorman, R., Henrich, J., & Vugt, M. V. (2009). Constraining free riding in public goods games: Designated solitary punishers can sustain human cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276(1655), 323–329.Google Scholar
  114. Olson, M. (1956). The logic of collective action: Public goods and the theory of groups. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  115. Pasternak, B., Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (1997). Sex, gender, and kinship: A cross-cultural perspective. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  116. Peterson, N. (1993). Demand sharing: Reciprocity and the pressure for generosity among foragers. American Anthropologist, 95(4), 860–874.Google Scholar
  117. Piantadosi, S. T., & Kidd, C. (2016). Extraordinary intelligence and the care of infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(25), 6874–6879.Google Scholar
  118. Pospisil, L. J. (1964). Law and societal structure among the Nunamiut Eskimo. In W. W. Goodenough (Ed.), Explorations in cultural anthropology: Essays in honor of George Peter Murdock (pp. 395–431). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  119. Price, T. D. (1995). Social inequality at the origins of agriculture. In T. D. Price & G. M. Feinman (Eds.), Foundations of social inequality (pp. 129–151). Boston: Springer.Google Scholar
  120. Price, M. E. (2003). Pro-community altruism and social status in a Shuar village. Human Nature, 14(2), 191–208.Google Scholar
  121. Price, T. D., & Feinman, G. M. (2010). Social inequality and the evolution of human social organization. In G. M. Feinman, T. D. Price, & G. M. Feinman (Eds.), Pathways to power (pp. 1–14). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  122. Price, M. E., & Van Vugt, M. (2014). The evolution of leader-follower reciprocity: The theory of service-for-prestige. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 363.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00363.Google Scholar
  123. Price, M. E., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2002). Punitive sentiment as an anti-free rider psychological device. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(3), 203–231.Google Scholar
  124. Quinn, N. (1977). Anthropological studies on women’s status. Annual Review of Anthropology, 6, 181–225.Google Scholar
  125. R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
  126. Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. (1922). Andaman islanders: A study in social anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  127. Richards, A. I. (1940). “Political system of the Bemba tribe, north-eastern Rhodesia. In M. Fortes and E. E. Evans-Pritchard (Eds.) African political systems (pp. 83–120). Published for the International Institute of African Languages & Cultures. London: Oxford University Press, H. Milford. http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=fq05-007.
  128. Richerson, P., & Henrich, J. (2012). Tribal social instincts and the cultural evolution of institutions to solve collective action problems. Cliodynamics, 3(1). 38–80.Google Scholar
  129. Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., Mathew, S., Newton, E. K., Naar, N., Newson, L., Ross, C., Smaldino, P. E., Waring, T. M., & Zefferman, M. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e30.Google Scholar
  130. Rodseth, L., Wrangham, R. W., Harrigan, A. M., & Smuts, B. B. (1991). The human community as a primate society. Current Anthropology, 32(3), 221–254.Google Scholar
  131. Rogers, E. S., & Taylor, J. G. (1981). Northern Ojibwa. In J. Helm (Ed.), Handbook of North American Indians, vol. 6: Subarctic (pp. 231–243). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution.Google Scholar
  132. Rosaldo, M. Z., Lamphere, L., & Bamberger, J. (Eds.). (1974). Woman, culture, and society. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  133. Roscoe, P. (2007). Intelligence, coalitional killing, and the antecedents of war. American Anthropologist, 109(3), 485–495.Google Scholar
  134. Roscoe, P. (2009). Social signaling and the organization of small-scale society: The case of contact-era New Guinea. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 16(2), 69–116.Google Scholar
  135. Sahlins, M. D. (1963). Poor man, rich man, big-man, chief: Political types in Melanesia and Polynesia. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 5(3), 285–303.Google Scholar
  136. Sapolsky, R. M. (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308, 648–652.Google Scholar
  137. Service, E. R. (1964). Primitive social organization. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  138. Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature, 15(4), 343–364.Google Scholar
  139. Smith, J. E., Gavrilets, S., Mulder, M. B., Hooper, P. L., Mouden, C. E., Nettle, D., Hauert, C., Hill, K., Perry, S., Pusey, A. E., van Vugt, M., & Smith, E. A. (2016). Leadership in mammalian societies: Emergence, distribution, power, and payoff. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 31(1), 54–66.Google Scholar
  140. Spencer, R. F. (1959). The North Alaskan Eskimo: A study in ecology and society. Bulletin of American Ethnology 171. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
  141. Spisak, B. R., Dekker, P. H., Krüger, M., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Warriors and peacekeepers: Testing a biosocial implicit leadership hypothesis of intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces. PLoS One, 7(1), e30399.Google Scholar
  142. Stanish, C. (2004). The evolution of chiefdoms: An economic anthropological model. In G. M. Feinman (Ed.), Archaeological perspectives on political economies (pp. 7–24). Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
  143. Sugiyama, L. S. (2004). Illness, injury, and disability among Shiwiar forager-horticulturalists: Implications of health-risk buffering for the evolution of human life history. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123(4), 371–389.Google Scholar
  144. Suzuki, R., & Shimodaira, H. (2006). Pvclust: An R package for assessing the uncertainty in hierarchical clustering. Bioinformatics, 22(12), 1540–1542.Google Scholar
  145. Suzuki, R., & Shimodaira, H. (2015). Pvclust: Hierarchical clustering with p-values via multiscale bootstrap resampling. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pvclust
  146. Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B: Methodological, 58(1), 267–288.Google Scholar
  147. Tiger, L. (1970). Dominance in human societies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1(1), 287–306.Google Scholar
  148. Tiger, L., & Fox, R. (1971). The imperial animal. New York: Holt, Rinehart, Winston.Google Scholar
  149. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. In W. G. Runciman, J. M. Smith, & R. I. M. Dunbar (Eds.), Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and man (pp. 119–143). Proceedings of the British Academy, Vol. 88. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  150. Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Price, M. E. (2006). Cognitive adaptations for n-person exchange: The evolutionary roots of organizational behavior. Managerial and Decision Economics, 27(2–3), 103–129.Google Scholar
  151. Turney, P. D., & Pantel, P. (2010). From frequency to meaning: Vector space models of semantics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 37, 141–188.Google Scholar
  152. Van Lange, P. A., Balliet, D. P., Parks, C. D., & Van Vugt, M. (2014). Social dilemmas: Understanding human cooperation. USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  153. Van Vugt, M. (2006). Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 354–371.Google Scholar
  154. Van Vugt, M., & Kurzban, R. K. (2007). Cognitive and social adaptations for leadership and followership: Evolutionary game theory and group dynamics. In J. Forgas, W. von Hippel, & M. Haselton (Eds.) The evolution of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp. 229–244). Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology, Vol. 9. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  155. Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution: Some lessons from the past. American Psychologist, 63(3), 182–196.Google Scholar
  156. Vecsey, C. (1983). Traditional Ojibwa religion and its historical changes. Memoirs of the American Philosophical Society 152. Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  157. Vehrencamp, S. L. (1983). A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies. Animal Behaviour, 31(3), 667–682.Google Scholar
  158. von Rueden, C. R., & Jaeggi, A. V. (2016). Men’s status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201606800.Google Scholar
  159. von Rueden, C. R., & Van Vugt, M. (2015). Leadership in small-scale societies: Some implications for theory, research, and practice. Leadership Quarterly, 26, 978–990.Google Scholar
  160. von Rueden, C. R., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2008). The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(6), 402–415.Google Scholar
  161. von Rueden, C. R., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2010). Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1715), 2223–2232.Google Scholar
  162. von Rueden, C. R., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Stieglitz, J. (2014). Leadership in an egalitarian society. Human Nature, 25, 538–566.Google Scholar
  163. von Rueden, C. R., Alami, S., Kaplan, H., & Gurven, M. (2018). Sex differences in political leadership in an egalitarian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 402–411.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.005.Google Scholar
  164. Walker, R. S., Hill, K. R., Flinn, M. V., & Ellsworth, R. M. (2011). Evolutionary history of hunter-gatherer marriage practices. PLoS One, 6(4), e19066.Google Scholar
  165. Weber, M. (1978). Economy and society: An outline of interpretive sociology (Vol. 1). Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  166. Wiessner, P. (2002). Hunting, healing, and Hxaro exchange: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/'hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(6), 407–436.Google Scholar
  167. Wiessner, P. (2010). The power of one? Big men revisited. In K. J. Vaughn, J. W. Eerkens, & J. Kantner (Eds.), The evolution of leadership: Transitions in decision making from small-scale to middle-range societies (pp. 195–222). Santa Fe: SAR Press.Google Scholar
  168. Wilson, D. S., Near, D., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 285–299.Google Scholar
  169. Wilson, D. S., Timmel, J. J., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Cognitive cooperation. Human Nature, 15(3), 225–250.Google Scholar
  170. Zaleznik, A. (1977). Managers and leaders: Are they different? Harvard Business Review, 55(3), 67–78.Google Scholar
  171. Zou, H., & Hastie, T. (2005). Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B: Methodological, 67(2), 301–320.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyWashington State UniversityVancouverUSA

Personalised recommendations