Human Nature

, Volume 24, Issue 4, pp 402–413 | Cite as

Menstrual Cycle Effects on Attitudes toward Romantic Kissing

Article

Abstract

Hormonal changes associated with the human menstrual cycle have been previously found to affect female mate preference, whereby women in the late follicular phase of their cycle (i.e., at higher risk of conception) prefer males displaying putative signals of underlying genetic fitness. Past research also suggests that romantic kissing is utilized in human mating contexts to assess potential mating partners. The current study examined whether women in their late follicular cycle phase place greater value on kissing at times when it might help serve mate assessment functions. Using an international online questionnaire, results showed that women in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle felt that kissing was more important at initial stages of a relationship than women in the luteal phase of their cycle. Furthermore, it was found that estimated progesterone levels were a significant negative predictor for these ratings.

Keywords

Menstrual cycle Romantic kissing Mate assessment Relationships 

References

  1. Baker, R. R., & Bellis, M. A. (1994). Human Sperm Competition: Copulation, Masturbation and Infidelity. Springer.Google Scholar
  2. Booth, A., & Dabbs, J. M. (1993). Testosterone and men’s marriages. Social Forces, 72(2), 463. doi:10.2307/2579857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chiazze, L., Brayer, F. T., Macisco, J. J., Parker, M. P., & Duffy, B. J. (1968). The length and variability of the human menstrual cycle. Journal of the American Medical Association, 203(6), 89–92.Google Scholar
  4. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Women’s attractiveness judgments of self-resembling faces change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 47(4), 379–83. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.006.Google Scholar
  5. Doty, R. L., Snyder, P. J., Huggins, G. R., & Lowry, L. D. (1981). Endocrine, cardiovascular, and psychological correlated of olfactory sensitivity changes during the human menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95(1), 45–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Durante, K. M., & Li, N. P. (2009). Oestradiol level and opportunistic mating in women. Biology Letters, 5(2), 179–82. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Durham, T. M., Malloy, T., & Hodges, E. D. (1993). Halitosis: knowing when “bad breath” signals systemic disease. Geriatrics, 48(8), 55–9. doi:10.1515/semi.1974.12.3.189.Google Scholar
  8. Fehring, R. J., Schneider, M., & Raviele, K. (2006). Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 35(3), 376–84. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6909.2006.00051.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone and prolactin are associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42(4), 399–413. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Foerster, K., Delhey, K., Johnsen, A., Lifjeld, J. T., & Kempenaers, B. (2003). Females increase offspring heterozygosity and fitness through extra-pair matings. Nature, 425(6959), 714–7. doi:10.1038/nature01969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Folstad, I., & Karter, A. J. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. American Naturalist, 139(3), 603–622.Google Scholar
  12. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–933. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15(3), 203–207. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., Miller, R. D., & Olp, J. J. (2006). Major histocompatibility complex alleles, sexual responsivity, and unfaithfulness in romantic couples. Psychological Science, 17(10), 830–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01789.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Haselton, M. G., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Conditional expression of women’s desires and men’s mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 49(4), 509–18. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.10.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Haselton, M. G., & Miller, G. F. (2006). Women’s fertility across the cycle increases the short-term attractiveness of creative intelligence. Human Nature, 17(1), 50–73. doi:10.1007/s12110-006-1020-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Havlicek, J., Roberts, S. C., & Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant male odour: effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biology Letters, 1(3), 256–9. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. New York: Aldine.Google Scholar
  19. Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G. J. (2007). Sex differences in romantic kissing among college students: An evolutionary perspective. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(3), 612–631.Google Scholar
  20. Hugill, N., Fink, B., & Neave, N. (2010). The role of human body movements in mate selection. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(1), 66–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., Debruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Smith, M. J. L., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2005a). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behavior, 48(3), 283–90. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.03.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jones, B. C., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Cornwell, R. E., Feinberg, D. R., Tiddeman, B. P., et al. (2005b). Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1561), 347–54. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & Law Smith, M. J. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 78–84. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9268-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lens, I., Driesmans, K., Pandelaere, M., & Janssens, K. (2011). Would male conspicuous consumption capture the female eye? Menstrual cycle effects on women’s attention to status products. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 4–7. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.06.004
  25. Lenton, E. A., Landgren, B., & Sexton, L. (1984). Normal variation in the length of the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle: identification of the short luteal phase. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 91(7), 685–689. doi:10.1111/j.1471-0528.1984.tb04831.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & Burriss, R. P. (2007). Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 51(5), 633–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Manning, J. T., Barley, L., Walton, J. C., Lewis-Jones, D. I., Trivers, R. L., Singh, D., Thornhill, R., et al. (2000). The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes? Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(3), 163–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Meston, C. M., & Frohlich, P. F. (2000). The neurobiology of sexual function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(11), 1012–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Nicholson, B. (1984). Does kissing aid human bonding by semiochemical addiction? British Journal of Dermatology, 3, 623–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pause, B. M., Sojka, B., Krauel, K., Fehm-Wolfsdorf, G., & Ferstl, R. (1996). Olfactory information processing during the course of the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 44(1), 31–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., & Minamisawa, R. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399(6738), 741–2. doi:10.1038/21557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pillsworth, E. G., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2004). Ovulatory shifts in female sexual desire. Journal of Sex Research, 41(1), 55–65. doi:10.1080/00224490409552213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pollet, T. V., van der Meij, L., Cobey, K. D., & Buunk, A. P. (2011). Testosterone levels and their associations with lifetime number of opposite sex partners and remarriage in a large sample of American elderly men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 60(1), 72–77. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.03.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 388–397. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Regan, P. C. (1996). Rhythms of desire: The association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5, 145–156.Google Scholar
  36. Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199–226. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Roberts, S. C., & Little, A. C. (2008). Good genes, complementary genes and human mate preferences. Genetica, 134(1), 31–43. doi:10.1007/s10709-008-9254-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rule, N. O., Rosen, K. S., Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2011). Mating interest improves women’s accuracy in judging male sexual orientation. Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/0956797611412394.Google Scholar
  39. Stricker, R., Eberhart, R., Chevailler, M.-C., Quinn, F. A., Bischof, P., & Stricker, R. (2006). Establishment of detailed reference values for luteinizing hormone, follicle stimulating hormone, estradiol, and progesterone during different phases of the menstrual cycle on the Abbott ARCHITECT analyzer. Clinical Chemistry and Laboratory Medicine, 44(7), 883–7. doi:10.1515/CCLM.2006.160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). The scent of symmetry: A human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(3), 175–201. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00005-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wedekind, C., Seebeck, T., Bettens, F., & Paepke, A. J. (1995). MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 260(1359), 245–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Wilcox, A. J., Dunson, D. B., Weinberg, C. R., Trussell, J., & Baird, D. D. (2001). Likelihood of conception with a single act of intercourse: providing benchmark rates for assessment of post-coital contraceptives. Contraception, 63(4), 211–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wlodarski, R. & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2013) Examining the possible functions of kissing in romantic relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0190-1.

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations