Human Nature

, Volume 23, Issue 2, pp 133–148 | Cite as

Women Who Know Their Place

Sex-Based Differences in Spatial Abilities and Their Evolutionary Significance
Article

Abstract

Differences between men and women in the performance of tests designed to measure spatial abilities are explained by evolutionary psychologists in terms of adaptive design. The Hunter-Gatherer Theory of Spatial Ability suggests that the adoption of a hunter-gatherer lifestyle (assuming a sexual division of labor) created differential selective pressure on the development of spatial skills in men and women and, therefore, cognitive differences between the sexes. Here, we examine a basic spatial skill—wayfinding (the ability to plan routes and navigate a landscape)—in men and women in a natural, real-world setting as a means of testing the proposition that sex-based differences in spatial ability exist outside of the laboratory. Our results indicate that when physical differences are accounted for, men and women with equivalent experience perform equally well at complex navigation tasks in a real-world setting. We conclude that experience, gendered patterns of activity, and self-assessment are contributing factors in producing previously reported differences in spatial ability.

Keywords

Evolutionary psychology Spatial cognition Gender Wayfinding 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research would not have been possible without the support of the AHRC Centre for the Evolution of Cultural Diversity, Institute of Archaeology, UCL, with special thanks to Dr. James Steele (Director, CECD), and the Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Cambridge University. The help and advice of organizers of the 2009 Scottish 6-Day Orienteering Festival is gratefully acknowledged, with special thanks to Mr. Gareth Bryan-Jones. A.K. was supported by a Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship. The authors would also like to acknowledge Prof. F. Grine, Dr. Jay Stock, and Dr. Tom Smulders for their comments during the preparation of this manuscript.

References

  1. Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  3. Barnfield, A. M. C. (1999). Development of sex differences in spatial memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89(1), 339–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beatty, W. W. (2002). Sex difference in geographical knowledge: driving experience is not essential. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(6), 804–810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bosco, A., Longoni, A. M., & Vecchi, T. (2004). Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(5), 519–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 551–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Choi, J., McKillop, E., Ward, M., & L’Hirondelle, N. (2006). Sex-specific relationships between route-learning strategies and abilities in a large-scale environment. Environment and Behavior, 38(6), 791–801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dabbs, J. M., Chang, E. L., Strong, R. A., & Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(2), 89–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eals, M., & Silverman, I. (1994). The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(2), 95–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18, 850–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 195–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(6), 473–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gilmartin, P. P., & Patton, J. C. (1984). Comparing the sexes on spatial abilities: map-use skills. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 74(4), 605–619. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8306.1984.tb01477.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human wayfinding and cognitive maps. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behaviour (pp. 5–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Golledge, R. G. (2003). Human wayfinding and cognitive maps. In M. Rockman & J. Steele (Eds.), Colonization of unfamiliar landscapes: the archaeology of adaptation (pp. 25–43). London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  17. Golledge, R. G., Ruggles, A. J., Pellegrino, J. W., & Gale, N. D. (1993). Integrating route knowledge in an unfamiliar neighborhood: along and across route experiments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13(4), 293–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34(2), 151–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Istomin, K. V., & Dwyer, M. J. (2009). Finding the way: a critical discussion of anthropological theories of human spatial orientation with reference to reindeer herders of Northeastern Europe and Western Siberia. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 29–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. James, T. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). Sex differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array: location-shifts versus location-exchanges. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(3), 155–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Janzen, G., Jansen, C., & van Turennout, M. (2007). Memory consolidation of landmarks in good navigators. Hippocampus, 18(1), 40–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Johansen, B. T. (1997). Thinking in orienteering. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 13, 38–46.Google Scholar
  23. Jones, C. M., Braithwaite, V. A., & Healy, S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(3), 403–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kitchin, R. M. (1994). Cognitive maps: what are they and why study them? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14(1), 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11–12), 765–779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16(2), 137–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lawton, C. A. (2001). Gender and regional differences in spatial referents used in direction giving. Sex Roles, 44(5–6), 321–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Levy, L. J., Astur, R. S., & Frick, K. M. (2005). Men and women differ in object memory but not performance of a virtual radial maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(4), 853–862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479–1498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lobben, A. K. (2004). Tasks, strategies, and cognitive processes associated with navigational map reading: a review perspective. The Professional Geographer, 56(2), 270–281.Google Scholar
  31. Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior (pp. 125–151). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Maguire, A. M., Spiers, H. J., Good, C. D., Hartley, T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Burgess, N. (2003). Navigation expertise and the human hippocampus: a structural brain imaging analysis. Hippocampus, 13(2), 250–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Malinowski, J. C. (2001). Mental rotation and real-world wayfinding. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92(1), 19–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Malinowski, J. C., & Gillespie, W. T. (2001). Individual differences in performance on a large-scale, real-world wayfinding task. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21(1), 73–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mau, W. C., & Bikos, L. H. (2000). Educational and vocational aspirations of minority and female students: a longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling and Development, 78(2), 186–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McBurney, D. H., Gaulin, S. J. C., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). Superior spatial memory of women: stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(3), 165–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G., & Self, C. M. (1999). Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 89(3), 515–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Myrvold, B. (1996). Is it possible to find a “best” route? A look at accuracy and significance in route choice comparison. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 12, 19–36.Google Scholar
  39. Nadolne, M. J., & Stringer, A. Y. (2001). Ecologic validity in neuropsychological assessment: prediction of wayfinding. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(6), 675–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Neave, N., Hamilton, C., Hutton, L., Tildesley, N., & Pickering, A. T. (2005). Some evidence of a female advantage in object location memory using ecologically valid stimuli. Human Nature, 16(2), 146–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. New, J., Krasnow, M. M., Truxaw, D., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (2007). Spatial adaptations for plant foraging: women excel and calories count. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 274(1626), 2679–2684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. O’Brien, V., Martinez-Pons, M., & Kopala, M. (1999). Mathematics self-efficacy, ethnic identity, gender, and career interests related to mathematics and science. The Journal of Educational Research, 92(4), 231–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical problem-solving: a path-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 193–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Postma, A., Jager, G., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, H. P. F., & van Honk, J. (2004). Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain and Cognition, 54(1), 24–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Lehmann, W. (2002). Girls’ spatial abilities: charting the contributions of experiences and attitudes in different academic groups. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 245–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Randhawa, B. S. (1994). Self-efficacy in mathematics, attitudes, and achievements of boys and girls from restricted samples in 2 countries. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(2), 1011–1018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Reis, S. M., & Park, S. (2001). Gender differences in high-achieving students in math and science. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 25(1), 52–73.Google Scholar
  48. Rilea, S. L., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Boles, D. (2004). Sex differences in spatial ability: a lateralization of function approach. Brain and Cognition, 56(3), 332–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(4), 351–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Saucier, D. M., McCreary, D. R., & Saxberg, J. K. J. (2002). Does gender role socialization mediate sex differences in mental rotations? Personality and Individual Differences, 32(6), 1101–1111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Shelton, A. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Neural correlates of individual differences in spatial learning strategies. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 442–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., & Olshansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(3), 201–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Silverman, I., and Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: evolutionnary theory and data. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and L. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 487–503). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Tottenham, L. S., Saucier, D., Elias, L., & Gutwin, C. (2003). Female advantage for spatial location memory in both static and dynamic environments. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 381–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Voyer, D., Postma, A., Brake, B., & Imperato-McGinley, J. (2007). Gender differences in object location memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(1), 23–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Whitaker, L. A., & Cuqlock-Knopp, G. (1992). Navigation in off-road environments: orienteering interviews. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 8, 55–71.Google Scholar
  58. Wynn, T. G., Tierson, F. D., & Palmer, C. T. (1996). Evolution of sex differences in spatial cognition. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 39, 11–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Département d’AnthropologieUniversité de MontréalMontréalCanada
  2. 2.AHRC Centre for the Evolution of Cultural Diversity, Institute of ArchaeologyUniversity College of LondonLondonUK
  3. 3.Santa Fe InstituteSanta FeUSA
  4. 4.Department of Social and Developmental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations