Human Nature

, Volume 19, Issue 4, pp 389–413 | Cite as

Autism as the Low-Fitness Extreme of a Parentally Selected Fitness Indicator

Article

Abstract

Siblings compete for parental care and feeding, while parents must allocate scarce resources to those offspring most likely to survive and reproduce. This could cause offspring to evolve traits that advertise health, and thereby attract parental resources. For example, experimental evidence suggests that bright orange filaments covering the heads of North American coot chicks may have evolved for this fitness-advertising purpose. Could any human mental disorders be the equivalent of dull filaments in coot chicks—low-fitness extremes of mental abilities that evolved as fitness indicators? One possibility is autism. Suppose that the ability of very young children to charm their parents evolved as a parentally selected fitness indicator. Young children would vary greatly in their ability to charm parents, that variation would correlate with underlying fitness, and autism could be the low-fitness extreme of this variation. This view explains many seemingly disparate facts about autism and leads to some surprising and testable predictions.

Keywords

Autism Sexual selection Parental selection Sibling rivalry Fitness indicator 

References

  1. Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Education, 13(2), 232–240.Google Scholar
  2. Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36(2), 199–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Badcock, C. (1989). The evolutionary dimension of the oral phase. Across Species Comparisons and Psychiatry Newsletter, 2(3), 6–7.Google Scholar
  5. Badcock, C., & Crespi, B. (2006). Imbalanced genomic imprinting in brain development: an evolutionary basis for the aetiology of autism. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 19(4), 1007–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25(1), 63–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 248–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Autism: a window onto the development of the social and the analytic brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28(1), 109–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bauman, M. L., & Kemper, T. L. (2005). Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 183–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(6), 443–452.Google Scholar
  11. Bella, H., & Al-Almaie, S. M. (2005). Do children born before and after adequate birth intervals do better at school? Journal of Tropical Pediatrics, 51(5), 265–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bongaarts, J. (1983). Fertility, biology and behavior. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  13. Bradbury, J. W., & Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of animal communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
  14. Brown, W. M., & Consedine, N. S. (2004). Just how happy is the happy puppet? An emotion signaling and kinship theory perspective on the behavioral phenotype of children with Angelman syndrome. Medical Hypotheses, 63(3), 377–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children. JAMA, 285(24), 3093–3099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Clark, M. M., Malenfant, S. A., Winter, D. A., & Galef, B. G. J. (1990). Fetal uterine position affects copulation and scent marking by adult male gerbils. Physiology & Behavior, 47(2), 301–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clutton-Brock, T. H., Albon, S. D., & Guinness, F. E. (1981). Parental investment in male and female offspring in polygynous mammals. Nature (London), 289(5797), 487–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cockburn, A. (1994). Adaptive sex allocation by brood reduction in antechinuses. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 35(1), 53–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cohen, D., Pichard, N., Tordjman, S., Baumann, C., Burglen, L., Excoffier, E., et al. (2005). Specific genetic disorders and autism: clinical contribution towards their identification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 103–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Constantino, J. N., Lajonchere, C., Lutz, M., Gray, T., Abbacchi, A., McKenna, K., et al. (2006). Autistic social impairment in the siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(2), 294–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Crow, J. F. (2000). The origins, patterns and implications of human spontaneous mutation. Nature Reviews Genetics, 1(1), 40–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1995). Discriminative parental solicitude and the relevance of evolutionary models to the analysis of motivational systems. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1269–1286). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Davies, W., Isles, A. R., Burgoyne, P. S., & Wilkinson, L. S. (2006). X-linked imprinting: effects on brain and behaviour. Bioessays, 28(1), 35–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Devinney, B. J., Berman, C. M., & Rasmussen, K. L. R. (1998). Weaning and maternal responsiveness to distress calling among free-ranging rhesus monkeys on Cayo Santiago. American Journal of Primatology, 45(2), 176–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dyches, T. T., Wilder, L. K., Sudweeks, R. R., Obiakor, F. E., & Algozzine, B. (2004). Multicultural issues in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 211–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Francis, K. (2005). Autism interventions: a critical update. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47(7), 493–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Furlow, F. B. (1997). Human neonatal cry quality as an honest signal of fitness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(3), 175–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., Eilers, L., & Ghaziuddin, N. (1992). Brief report: autism and herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(1), 107–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gibson, M. A., & Mace, R. (2003). Strong mothers bear more sons in rural Ethiopia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 270(Supplement 1), S108–S109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Glasson, E. J., Bower, C., Petterson, B., de Klerk, N., Chaney, G., & Hallmayer, J. F. (2004). Perinatal factors and the development of autism: a population study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(6), 618–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Godfray, H. C. J. (1995). Evolutionary theory of parent-offspring conflict. Nature, 376(6536), 133–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Haig, D., & Wharton, R. (2003). Prader-Willi syndrome and the evolution of human childhood. American Journal of Human Biology, 15(3), 320–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Harrell, W. A. (2005). Physical attractiveness of children and parental supervision in grocery stores: An evolutionary explanation of the neglect of ugly kids. Paper presented at the 16th Annual Warren E. Kalbach Population Conference: Population Concerns of the Western Provinces, March 11, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada.Google Scholar
  35. Hasson, O. (1989). Amplifiers and the handicap principle in sexual selection a different emphasis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 235(1281), 383–406.Google Scholar
  36. Hasson, O. (1997). Towards a general theory of biological signaling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 185(2), 139–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Horwood, L. J., Darlow, B. A., & Mogridge, N. (2001). Breast milk feeding and cognitive ability at 7–8 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 84(1), F23–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Houle, D. (1992). Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics, 130(1), 195–204.Google Scholar
  39. Houle, D., & Kondrashov, A. S. (2002). Coevolution of costly mate choice and condition-dependent display of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 269(1486), 97–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother nature. New York: Ballantine.Google Scholar
  41. Keller, M. C., & Miller, G. (2006). Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: which evolutionary genetic models work best? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 385–452.Google Scholar
  42. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895–908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kolevzon, A., Gross, R., & Reichenberg, A. (2007). Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism: a review and integration of findings. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 326–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kolliker, M., & Richner, H. (2001). Parent-offspring conflict and the genetics of offspring solicitation and parental response. Animal Behaviour, 62(3), 395–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Konner, M. (2005). Hunter-Gatherer infancy and childhood: The !Kung and others. In B. S. Hewlett, M. E. & Lamb (Eds.), Hunter–gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, & cultural perspectives (pp. 19–64, 1st ed.). New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction.Google Scholar
  46. Lee, P. C. (1996). The meanings of weaning: growth, lactation, and life history. Evolutionary Anthropology, 5(3), 87–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Locke, J. L. (2006). Parental selection of vocal behavior. Human Nature, 17, 155–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lummaa, V. (2001). Reproductive investment in pre-industrial humans: the consequences of offspring number, gender and survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 268(1480), 1977–1983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lyon, B. E., Eadie, J. M., & Hamilton, L. D. (1994). Parental choice selects for ornamental plumage in American coot chicks. Nature, 371(6494), 240–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mace, R., & Sear, R. (1997). Birth interval and the sex of children in a traditional African population: an evolutionary analysis. Journal of Biosocial Science, 29(4), 499–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239–1245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Malm, K., & Jensen, P. (1997). Weaning and parent-offspring conflict in the domestic dog. Ethology, 103(8), 653–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Manning, J. T., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Sanders, G. (2001). The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 43(3), 160–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Michod, R. E., & Hasson, O. (1990). On the evolution of reliable indicators of fitness. American Naturalist, 135(6), 788–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature (1st ed.). New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  56. Miller, G. F., & Tal, I. R. (2007). Schizotypy versus openness and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1–3), 317–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Mock, D. W., & Parker, G. A. (1997). The evolution of sibling rivalry. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Moore, T., & Haig, D. (1991). Genomic imprinting in mammalian development—A parental tug-of-war. Trends in Genetics, 7(2), 45–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Oddy, W. H. (2001). Breastfeeding protects against illness and infection in infants and children: a review of the evidence. Breastfeeding Review, 9(2), 11–18.Google Scholar
  60. Peters, S. U., Beaudet, A. L., Madduri, N., & Bacino, C. A. (2004). Autism in Angelman syndrome: implications for autism research. Clinical Genetics, 66(6), 530–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pomiankowski, A., & Moller, A. P. (1995). A resolution of the lek paradox. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 260(1357), 21–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Pritchard, J. K. (2001). Are rare variants responsible for susceptibility to complex diseases? American Journal of Human Genetics, 69(1), 124–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Prokosch, M. D., Yeo, R. A., & Miller, G. F. (2005). Intelligence tests with higher g-loadings show higher correlations with body symmetry: evidence for a general fitness factor mediated by developmental stability. Intelligence, 33(2), 203–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Queller, D. C. (1994). Male–female conflict and parent–offspring conflict. American Naturalist, 144, S84–S99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Quinlan, R. J., Quinlan, M. B., & Flinn, M. V. (2003). Parental investment and age at weaning in a Caribbean village. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(1), 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Redcay, E., & Courchesne, E. (2005). When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biological Psychiatry, 58(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Reichenberg, A., Gross, R., Weiser, M., Bresnahan, M., Silverman, J., Harlap, S., et al. (2006). Advancing paternal age and autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(9), 1026–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Risch, N., Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L., Nouri, N., Hinds, D., Hallmayer, J., et al. (1999). A genomic screen of autism: evidence for a multilocus etiology. American Journal of Human Genetics, 65(2), 493–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rodier, P. M., Bryson, S. E., & Welch, J. P. (1997). Minor malformations and physical measurements in autism: data from Nova Scotia. Teratology, 55(5), 319–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rowe, L., & Houle, D. (1996). The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 263(1375), 1415–1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rushton, J. P. (1996). Race, genetics, and human reproductive strategies. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 122(1), 21–53.Google Scholar
  72. Saino, N., Ninni, P., Calza, S., Martinelli, R., De Bernardi, F., & Moller, A. P. (2000). Better red than dead: carotenoid-based mouth coloration reveals infection in barn swallow nestlings. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 267(1438), 57–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Sebat, J., Lakshmi, B., Malhotra, D., Troge, J., Lese-Martin, C., Walsh, T., et al. (2007). Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science, 316(5823), 445–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Shaner, A., Miller, G., & Mintz, J. (2004). Schizophrenia as one extreme of a sexually selected fitness indicator. Schizophrenia Research, 70(1), 101–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Shaner, A., Miller, G., & Mintz, J. (2007). Mental disorders as catastrophic failures of mating intelligence. In G. Geher, G. & Miller (Eds.), Mating intelligence: Sex, relationships, and the mind's reproductive system (pp. 193–223). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  76. Shavelle, R. M., & Strauss, D. (1998). Comparative mortality of persons with autism in California, 1980–1996. Journal of Insurance Medicine, 30(4), 220–225.Google Scholar
  77. Short, A. B., & Schopler, E. (1988). Factors relating to age of onset in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(2), 207–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Skuse, D. H. (2000). Imprinting, the X-chromosome, and the male brain: explaining sex differences in the liability to autism. Pediatric Research, 47(1), 9–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Soltis, J. (2004). The signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 443–458 (discussion 459–490).Google Scholar
  80. Stern, J. M., Konner, M., Herman, T. N., & Reichlin, S. (1986). Nursing behaviour, prolactin and postpartum amenorrhoea during prolonged lactation in American and !Kung mothers. Clinical Endocrinology, 25(3), 247–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Stromland, K., Nordin, V., Miller, M., Akerstrom, B., & Gillberg, C. (1994). Autism in thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 36(4), 351–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Tang, C. H., Wu, M. P., Liu, J. T., Lin, H. C., & Hsu, C. C. (2006). Delayed parenthood and the risk of cesarean delivery—Is paternal age an independent risk factor? Birth, 33(1), 18–26.Google Scholar
  83. Tanoue, Y., & Oda, S. (1989). Weaning time of children with infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3), 425–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Taylor, H. W., Vazquez-Geffroy, M., Samuels, S. J., & Taylor, D. M. (1999). Continuously recorded suckling behaviour and its effect on lactational amenorrhoea. Journal of Biosocial Science, 31(3), 289–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Thapa, S., Short, R. V, & Potts, M. (1988). Breast feeding, birth spacing and their effects on child survival. Nature, 335(6192), 679–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Tomkins, J. L., Radwan, J., Kotiaho, J. S., & Tregenza, T. (2004). Genic capture and resolving the lek paradox. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19(6), 323–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14(1), 249–264.Google Scholar
  88. Veenstra-Vanderweele, J., Christian, S. L., & Cook, E. H. (2004). Autism as a paradigmatic complex genetic disorder. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet, 5, 379–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Vestergaard, M., Obel, C., Henriksen, T. B., Sorensen, H. T., Skajaa, E., & Ostergaard, J. (1999). Duration of breastfeeding and developmental milestones during the latter half of infancy. Acta Paediatrica, 88(12), 1327–1332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. World Health Organization (1998). The World Health Organization multinational study of breast-feeding and lactational amenorrhea. II. Factors associated with the length of amenorrhea. Fertility and Sterility, 70(3), 461–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Zhao, X., Leotta, A., Kustanovich, V., Lajonchere, C., Geschwind, D. H., Law, K., et al. (2007). A unified genetic theory for sporadic and inherited autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(31), 12831–12836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© United States Department of Veterans Affairs 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health (116A)Veterans Affairs Healthcare System and the University of California Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of New MexicoAlbuquerqueUSA
  3. 3.Departments of Psychiatry and Epidemiology/BiostatisticsUniversity of Texas Health Science Center San AntonioSan AntonioUSA

Personalised recommendations