Advertisement

Distinct effects of visual and auditory temporal processing training on reading and reading-related abilities in Chinese children with dyslexia

  • Li-Chih WangEmail author
  • Duo Liu
  • Zhengye Xu
Article

Abstract

This study aimed to examine the effects of temporal processing training on the reading abilities of Chinese children with dyslexia. In total, 69 Chinese children with dyslexia in grades three through six were recruited in Taiwan. The children were divided into the following three equal groups: (1) auditory temporal processing training group, (2) visual temporal processing training group, and (3) control group with no specific training. The participants in both training groups received instruction with identical durations (30–40 min), intensities (12 times in total), and frequencies (three to four times per week). The participants in the control group were asked to independently surf some specified websites using devices similar to those used by the two experimental groups for an identical duration, intensity, and frequency. Our results indicated that the two groups who received temporal processing training exhibited significant correlations among Chinese character reading, rapid naming, and corresponding reading-related abilities, while visual temporal processing served as a significant predictor of Chinese character reading ability even if all background data, reading-related abilities, and auditory temporal processing were introduced first. Additionally, significant interactions were found between the Groups and Tested sessions in all the measures, except for phonological awareness, confirming the distinct effects of different temporal processing on most measures involved in this study. Further simple main effects revealed that only those who received the visual temporal processing training gained benefits in the corresponding reading-related ability (i.e., orthographic knowledge) and far-transfer to Chinese character reading.

Keywords

Auditory temporal processing training Chinese reading Dyslexia Visual temporal processing training 

Notes

Funding

The work described in this article was supported by the Departmental Research Seed Fund of Department of Special Education and Counselling at the Education University of Hong Kong.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alais, D., Cass, J., O’Shea, R. P., & Blake, R. (2010). Visual sensitivity underlying changes in visual consciousness. Current Biology, 20(15), 1362–1367.Google Scholar
  2. Basu, M., Krishnan, A., & Weber-Fox, C. (2010). Brainstem correlates of temporal auditory processing in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 13(1), 77–91.Google Scholar
  3. Becker, B. J. (1988). Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 41(2), 257–278.Google Scholar
  4. Becker, C., Elliott, M. A., & Lachmann, T. (2005). Evidence for impaired visuoperceptual organisation in developmental dyslexics and its relation to temporal processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(5), 499–522.Google Scholar
  5. Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high-risk for dyslexia: a longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1608–1620.Google Scholar
  6. Bretherton, L., & Holmes, V. M. (2003). The relationship between auditory temporal processing, phonemic awareness, and reading disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(3), 218–243.Google Scholar
  7. Breznitz, Z., & Misra, M. (2003). Speed of processing of the visual–orthographic and auditory–phonological systems in adult dyslexics: the contribution of “asynchrony” to word recognition deficits. Brain and Language, 85(3), 486–502.Google Scholar
  8. Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(6), 510–525.Google Scholar
  9. Caylak, E. (2011). The auditory temporal processing deficit theory in children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Pediatric Neurology, 09(02), 151–168.Google Scholar
  10. Chen, H. C., Chang, L. Y., Chiou, Y. S., Sung, Y. T., & Chang, K. E. (2011). Chinese orthographic database and its application in teaching Chinese characters. Bulletin of Educational Psychology, 43(Special Issue), 269–290.Google Scholar
  11. Chen, J. H., & Chen, H. Y. (2006). Raven’s progressive matrices-parallel manual. Taipei, Taiwan: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation.Google Scholar
  12. Cheung, H., Chung, K. K. H., Wong, S. W. L., McBride-Chang, C., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. S. H. (2009). Perception of tone and aspiration contrasts in Chinese children with dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(6), 726–733.Google Scholar
  13. Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Wong, S. W. L. (2013). General auditory processing, speech perception and phonological awareness skills in Chinese-English biliteracy. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(2), 202–222.Google Scholar
  14. Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W. L., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. S. H. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58(1), 15–35.Google Scholar
  15. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  16. Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(6), 1197–1211.Google Scholar
  17. Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222.Google Scholar
  18. DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: the diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
  19. Donohue, S. E., Woldorff, M. G., & Mitroff, S. R. (2010). Video game players show more precise multisensory temporal processing abilities. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(4), 1120–1129.Google Scholar
  20. Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(4), 460–493.Google Scholar
  21. Fortes, I. S., Paula, C. S., Oliveira, M. C., Bordin, I. A., de Jesus Mari, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2016). A cross-sectional study to assess the prevalence of DSM-5 specific learning disorders in representative school samples from the second to sixth grade in Brazil. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(2), 195–207.Google Scholar
  22. Fostick, L., Eshcoly, R., Shtibelman, H., Nehemia, R., & Levi, H. (2014). Efficacy of temporal processing training to improve phonological awareness among dyslexic and normal reading students. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1799–1807.Google Scholar
  23. Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., & Temple, E. (2007). Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25(3–4), 295–310.Google Scholar
  24. Georgiou, G. K., Protopapas, A., Papadopoulos, T. C., Skaloumbakas, C., & Parrila, R. (2010). Auditory temporal processing and dyslexia in an orthographically consistent language. Cortex, 46(10), 1330–1344.Google Scholar
  25. Halliday, L. F., Taylor, J. L., Millward, K. E., & Moore, D. R. (2012). Lack of generalization of auditory learning in typically developing children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55(1), 168–181.Google Scholar
  26. Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 525–532.Google Scholar
  27. Hoosain, R. (1992). Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In H. Chen & O. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 111–130). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  28. Huang, H. S. (2001). Chinese Character Recognition Test. Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Publishing, Co., Ltd.Google Scholar
  29. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829–6833.Google Scholar
  30. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464–480.Google Scholar
  31. Klein, R. M. (2002). Observations on the temporal correlates of reading failure. Reading and Writing, 15(1), 207–231.Google Scholar
  32. Landerl, K., & Willburger, E. (2010). Temporal processing, attention, and learning disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(5), 393–401.Google Scholar
  33. Li, X., Rayner, K., & Cave, K. R. (2009). On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 58(4), 525–552.Google Scholar
  34. Liu, C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2017). Cognitive and environmental correlates of rapid automatized naming in Chinese kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(4), 465–476.Google Scholar
  35. Liu, D., Chen, X., & Chung, K. K. H. (2015). Performance in a visual search task uniquely predicts reading abilities in third-grade Hong Kong Chinese children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(4), 307–324.Google Scholar
  36. Liu, D., Chen, X., & Wang, Y. (2016). The impact of visual-spatial attention on reading and spelling in Chinese children. Reading and Writing, 29(7), 1435–1447.Google Scholar
  37. Liu, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Zhang, Y., Li, H., Liu, H., Song, S., et al. (2017). Contribution of cognitive and linguistic skills to word-reading accuracy and fluency in Chinese. International Journal of Educational Research, 82, 75–90.Google Scholar
  38. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1–14.Google Scholar
  39. Mattingly, I. G. (1984). Reading, linguistic awareness, and language acquisition. In J. Downing & R. Valtin (Eds.), Language awareness and learning to read (pp. 9–25). New York, NY: Springer New York.Google Scholar
  40. McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K. K. Y., & Li, Y. (2004). Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(2), 93–111.Google Scholar
  41. McBride, C. A. (2016). Is Chinese special? Four aspects of Chinese literacy acquisition that might distinguish learning Chinese from learning alphabetic orthographies. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 523–549.Google Scholar
  42. Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291.Google Scholar
  43. Meng, X., Sai, X., Wang, C., Wang, J., Sha, S., & Zhou, X. (2005). Auditory and speech processing and reading development in Chinese school children: behavioural and ERP evidence. Dyslexia, 11(4), 292–310.Google Scholar
  44. Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods, 11(2), 364–386.Google Scholar
  45. Murphy, C. F. B., & Schochat, E. (2010). Generalization of temporal order detection skill learning: two experimental studies of children with dyslexia. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 43(4), 359–366.Google Scholar
  46. Norton, E. S., Black, J. M., Stanley, L. M., Tanaka, H., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Sawyer, C., & Hoeft, F. (2014). Functional neuroanatomical evidence for the double-deficit hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 61, 235–246.Google Scholar
  47. Pinheiro, F. H., & Capellini, S. A. (2010). Auditory training in students with learning disabilities. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 22(1), 49–54.Google Scholar
  48. Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 287–292.Google Scholar
  49. Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., & Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: a meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1055–1079.Google Scholar
  50. Ramus, F. (2001). Talk of two theories. Nature, 412(6845), 393–394.Google Scholar
  51. Ramus, F., & Ahissar, M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: the difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(1–2), 104–122.Google Scholar
  52. Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., & Bélanger, N. N. (2010). Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(6), 834–839.Google Scholar
  53. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1999). The role of phonological awareness, speech perception, and auditory temporal processing for dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 8(S3), S28–S34.Google Scholar
  54. Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 886–899.Google Scholar
  55. Stauffer, C. C., Haldemann, J., Troche, S. J., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2012). Auditory and visual temporal sensitivity: evidence for a hierarchical structure of modality-specific and modality-independent levels of temporal information processing. Psychological Research, 76(1), 20–31.Google Scholar
  56. Steinbrink, C., Zimmer, K., Lachmann, T., Dirichs, M., & Kammer, T. (2014). Development of rapid temporal processing and its impact on literacy skills in primary school children. Child Development, 85(4), 1711–1726.Google Scholar
  57. Strehlow, U., Haffner, J., Bischof, J., Gratzka, V., Parzer, P., & Resch, F. (2006). Does successful training of temporal processing of sound and phoneme stimuli improve reading and spelling? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(1), 19–29.Google Scholar
  58. Tallal, P. (2000). Experimental studies of language learning impairments: from research to remediation. In D. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 131–155). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  59. Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M. Y. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and spelling errors: keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(5), 426–452.Google Scholar
  60. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(5), 276–286.Google Scholar
  61. Tzeng, Y.-L., Hsu, C.-H., Lin, W.-H., & Lee, C.-Y. (2018). Impaired orthographic processing in Chinese dyslexic children: evidence from the lexicality effect on N400. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 85–100.Google Scholar
  62. Vlachos, F., Avramidis, E., Dedousis, G., Chalmpe, M., Ntalla, I., & Giannakopoulou, M. (2013). Prevalence and gender ratio of dyslexia in Greek adolescents and its association with parental history and brain injury. American Journal of Educational Research, 1(1), 22–25.Google Scholar
  63. Waber, D. P., Weiler, M. D., Wolff, P. H., Bellinger, D., Marcus, D. J., Ariel, R., et al. (2001). Processing of rapid auditory stimuli in school-age children referred for evaluation of learning disorders. Child Development, 72(1), 37–49.Google Scholar
  64. Wang, L.-C., Liu, D., Chen, J.-K., & Wu, Y.-C. (2018). Processing speed of dyslexia: the relationship between temporal processing and rapid naming in Chinese. Reading and Writing, 31(7), 1645–1668.Google Scholar
  65. Wang, L.-C., & Yang, H.-M. (2018). Temporal processing development in Chinese primary school-aged children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(3), 302–312.Google Scholar
  66. Wang, L. C. (2017). Effects of phonological training on the reading and reading-related abilities of Hong Kong children with dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1904.Google Scholar
  67. Williams, C., & Bever, T. (2010). Chinese character decoding: a semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589–605.Google Scholar
  68. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (2000). Naming-speed processes and developmental reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 322–324.Google Scholar
  69. Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387–407.Google Scholar
  70. Yang, L.-Y., Guo, J.-P., Richman, L. C., Schmidt, F. L., Gerken, K. C., & Ding, Y. (2013). Visual skills and Chinese reading acquisition: a meta-analysis of correlation evidence. Educational Psychology Review, 25(1), 115–143.Google Scholar
  71. Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, and reading development and impairment. Educational Psychology Review, 22(3), 323–338.Google Scholar
  72. Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2014). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, L1 Chinese, and L2 English reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1001–1013.Google Scholar
  73. Zukowski, A., & Treiman, R. (2013). Levels of phonological awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 95–112). London: Routledge.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Special Education and CounsellingThe Education University of Hong KongHong KongChina
  2. 2.Integrated Centre for WellbeingThe Education University of Hong KongHong KongChina

Personalised recommendations