Advertisement

Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 62, Issue 2, pp 120–134 | Cite as

Identifying high-functioning dyslexics: is self-report of early reading problems enough?

  • S. Hélène DeaconEmail author
  • Kathryn Cook
  • Rauno Parrila
Article

Abstract

We used a questionnaire to identify university students with self-reported difficulties in reading acquisition during elementary school (self-report; n = 31). The performance of the self-report group on standardized measures of word and non-word reading and fluency, passage comprehension and reading rate, and phonological awareness was compared to that of two other groups of university students: one with a recent diagnosis (diagnosed; n = 20) and one with no self-reported reading acquisition problems (comparison group; n = 33). The comparison group outperformed both groups with a history of reading difficulties (self-report and diagnosed) on almost all measures. The self-report and diagnosed groups performed similarly on most tasks, with the exception of untimed reading comprehension (better performance for diagnosed) and reading rate (better performance for self-report). The two recruitment methods likely sample from the same underlying population but identify individuals with different adaptive strategies.

Keywords

Compensated dyslexics High-functioning dyslexics Reading ability Recruitment 

References

  1. Brown, J. I., Fishco, V. V., & Hanna, G. (1993). The Nelson-Denny reading test. Itasca: Riverside Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  2. Bruck, M. (1993). Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16, 171–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Deacon, S. H., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Processing of derived forms in high-functioning dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Decker, S. N., Vogler, G. P., & Defries, J. C. (1989). Validity of self-reported reading disability by parents of reading-disabled control children. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 1, 327–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00077.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 209–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fink, R. P. (1998). Literacy development in successful men and women with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 311–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Finucci, J. M., Whitehouse, C. C., Isaacs, S. D., & Childs, B. (1984). Derivation and validation of a quantitative definition of specific reading disability for adults. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 2, 143–153.Google Scholar
  9. Flynn, J. M., & Rahbar, M. H. (1994). Prevalence of reading failure in boys compared with girls. Psychology in the Schools, 31, 66–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. French, F., & Mureika, J. (2002). Enhancing the experience of children and youth in today’s schools: The role of psychology in Canadian schools. A position paper. Retrieved from CPA website: http://www.cpa.ca/documents/School_2.pdf
  11. Gallagher, A. M., Laxon, V., Armstrong, E., & Frith, U. (1996). Phonological difficulties in high-functioning dyslexics. Reading and Writing: an Interdicsiplinary Journal, 8, 499–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Grant, A., Wilson, A., & Gottardo, A. (2007). The role of print exposure in reading skills of post-secondary students with and without reading disabilities. Exceptionality Education Canada, 17, 175–194.Google Scholar
  13. Hanley, J. R. (1997). Reading and spelling impairments in undergraduate students with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 22–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kemp, N., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2009). Phonological and orthographic spelling in high-functioning adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15, 105–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kirby, J. R., Silvestri, R., Allingham, B. H., Parrila, R., & La Fave, C. B. (2008). Learning strategies and study approaches of postsecondary students with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 85–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kitz, W. R., & Tarver, G. S. (1989). Comparison of dyslexic and nondyslexic adults on decoding and phonemic awareness tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 196–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kozey, M., & Siegel, L. S. (2008). Definitions of learning disabilities in Canadian provinces and territories. Canadian Psychology, 49, 162–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Leach, J., Scarborough, H., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Reliability and validity of the adult reading history questionnaire. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 286–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Leong, C. K. (1999). Phonological and morphological processing in adult students with learning/reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 224–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McGonnell, M., Parrila, R., & Deacon, S. H. (2007). The recruitment and description of university students who self-report difficulty acquiring early reading skills. Exceptionality Education Canada, 17, 155–174.Google Scholar
  23. Meyler, A., & Breznitz, Z. (2003). Processing of phonological, orthographic and cross-modal word representations among adult dyslexic and normal readers. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 785–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Milne, D. R., Nicholson, T., & Corballis, C. M. (2003). Lexical access and phonological decoding in adult dyslexic subtypes. Neuropsychology, 17, 362–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nation, K. (2005). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 248–265). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Parrila, R., Corkett, J., Kirby, J., & Hein, S. (2003). Adult Reading History Questionnaire-Revised. Unpublished questionnaire. University of AlbertaGoogle Scholar
  27. Parrila, R., Georgiou, G., & Corkett, J. (2007). University students with a significant history of reading difficulties: What is and what is not compensated? Exceptionality Education Canada, 17, 195–220.Google Scholar
  28. Perfetti, C. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(4), 461–469. doi: 10.1037/h0077013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ransby, M. J., & Swanson, H. L. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 538–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Savage, R., & Wolforth, J. (2007). An additive simple view of reading describes the performance of good and poor readers in higher education. Exceptionality Education Canada, 17, 243–268.Google Scholar
  32. Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. E., Fletcher, J., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of the Connecticut longitudinal study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J., Holahan, J. M., Shneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., Pugh, K. R., & Shaywitz, B. E. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104(6), 1351–1359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Siegel, L. S. (1999). Issues in the definition and diagnosis of learning disabilities: A perspective on Guckenberger v. Boston University. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 304–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  37. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  38. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). The comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  39. Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery test-revised-normative update. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • S. Hélène Deacon
    • 1
    Email author
  • Kathryn Cook
    • 1
  • Rauno Parrila
    • 2
  1. 1.Psychology Department Dalhousie UniversityLife Sciences CentreHalifaxCanada
  2. 2.Department of Educational Psychology 6-102 Education North EdmontonUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada

Personalised recommendations