Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 61, Issue 1, pp 111–135 | Cite as

Orthographic processing efficiency in developmental dyslexia: an investigation of age and treatment factors at the sublexical level

  • Beth A. O’Brien
  • Maryanne Wolf
  • Lynne T. Miller
  • Maureen W. Lovett
  • Robin Morris
Article

Abstract

Reading fluency beyond decoding is a limitation to many children with developmental reading disorders. In the interest of remediating dysfluency, contributing factors need to be explored and understood in a developmental framework. The focus of this study is orthographic processing in developmental dyslexia, and how it may contribute to reading fluency. We investigated orthographic processing speed and accuracy by children identified with dyslexia that were enrolled in an intensive, fluency-based intervention using a timed visual search task as a tool to measure orthographic recognition. Results indicate both age and treatment effects, and delineate a link between rapid letter naming and efficient orthographic recognition. Orthographic efficiency was related to reading speed for passages, but not spelling performance. The role of orthographic learning in reading fluency and remediation is discussed.

Keywords

Developmental dyslexia Fluency Intervention Orthographic processing 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the children who participated in this research, and their families and teachers for their support, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. This study was conducted within a larger intervention project funded by NICHD (#HD30970, awarded to Morris, Lovett & Wolf).

References

  1. Adams, M. J. (1981). What good is orthographic redundancy? In O. J. L. Tzeng & H. Singer (Eds.), Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 197–221). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Apel, K. (2009). The acquisition of mental orthographic representations for reading and spelling development. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(1), 42–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bear, D. R., Invernizzi, M., Templeton, S., & Johnston, F. (2004). Words their way: Word study for phonics, vocabulary, and spelling instruction (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  5. Berninger, V. W. (1987). Global, component, and serial processing of printed words in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 387–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (1994). Multiple orthographic and phonological codes in literacy acquisition: An evolving research program. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 1: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 277–319). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  7. Berninger, V. W., & Wolf, B. J. (2009). Teaching students with dyslexia and dysgraphia: Lessons from Teaching and Science. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  8. Berninger, V. W., Yates, C., & Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing, 3, 115–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bosman, A. M. T., & Van Orden, G. C. (1997). Why spelling is more difficult than reading. In C. A. Perfetti (Ed.), Learning to spell (pp. 173–193). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Journal Reading and Writing, 5(1), 69–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Bruck, M. (1993). Component spelling skills of college students with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 16(3), 171–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Casco, C., & Prunetti, E. (1996). Visual search of good and poor readers: Effects with targets having single and combined features. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 1155–1167.Google Scholar
  14. Casco, C., Tressoldi, P. E., & Dellantonio, A. (1998). Visual selective attention and reading efficiency are related in children. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 34, 531–546.Google Scholar
  15. Castles, A., & Nation, K. (2008). Learning to be a good orthographic reader. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chall, J. (1983). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  17. Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehéricy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hénaff, M., et al. (2000). The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain, 123(2), 291–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Compton, D. L. (2000). Modeling the growth of decoding skills in first-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4(3), 219–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Compton, P. E., Grossenbacher, P. G., Posner, M. I., & Tucker, D. M. (1991). A cognitive-anatomical approach to attention in lexical access. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(4), 304–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Conrad, N., & Levy, B. (2007). Letter processing and the formation of memory representations in children with naming speed deficits. Reading and Writing, 20(3), 201–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Corcos, E., & Willows, D. (1993). The role of visual processing in good and poor readers’ utilization of orthographic information in letter strings. In S. Wright & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  22. Cornelissen, P., Bradley, L., Fowler, S., & Stein, J. (1994). What children see affects how they spell. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36(8), 716–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cornelissen, P., & Hansen, P. (1998). Motion detection, letter position encoding, and single word reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 48(1), 155–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P., Hutton, J., Evangelinou, V., & Stein, J. (1998). Magnocellular visual function and children’s single word reading. Vision Research, 38(3), 471–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 752–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10(2), 186–202.Google Scholar
  27. Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ecalle, J., Magnan, A., & Calmus, C. (2009). Lasting effects on literacy skills with a computer-assisted learning using syllabic units in low-progress readers. Computers & Education, 52(3), 554–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ehri, L. (1979). Linguistic insight: Threshold of reading acquisition. Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, 1, 63–114.Google Scholar
  30. Ehri, L. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22(6), 356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ehri, L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. Reading acquisition, 5, 107–143.Google Scholar
  32. Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ehri, L., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing, 7(3), 295–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Engelmann, S., & Bruner, E. (1988). Reading mastery I/II fast cycle: Teacher’s guide. Chicago: Science Research Associates.Google Scholar
  35. Engelmann, S., Johnson, G., Carnine, L., Meyer, L., Becher, W., & Eisele, J. (1988). Corrective Reading: Decoding strategies. Chicago: Science Research Associates.Google Scholar
  36. Facoetti, A., & Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39(4), 352–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. (2000). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36(1), 109–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Gerber, M. (1984). Techniques to teach generalizable spelling skills. Academic Therapy, 20(1), 49–58.Google Scholar
  40. Geva, E., & Willows, D. (1994). Orthographic knowledge is orthographic knowledge is orthographic knowledge. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I. Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 359–380). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  41. Geyer, L. (1977). Recognition and confusion of the lowercase alphabet. Perception & Psychophysics, 22(5), 487–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Gibson, E., Pick, A., Osser, H., & Hammond, M. (1962). The role of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in the perception of words. The American Journal of Psychology, 75, 554–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Henderson, E. H. (1992). The interface of lexical competence and knowledge of written words. In S. Templeton & D. Bear (Eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy: A memorial festschrift for Edward H. Henderson (pp. 1–30). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  44. Hultquist, A. (1997). Orthographic processing abilities of adolescents with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 89–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Invernizzi, M. (1992). The vowel and what follows: a phonological frame of orthographic analysis. In S. Templeton & D. Bear (Eds.), The development of orthographic knowledge: Foundations of literacy (pp. 105–136). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  46. Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Wide range achievement test-revised. Wilmington: Jastak Associates.Google Scholar
  47. Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 719–729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kame’enui, E., Simmons, D., Good, R., & Harn, B. (2001). The use of fluency-based measures in early identification and evaluation of intervention efficacy in schools. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, Fluency, and the Brain (pp. 307–331). Timonium: York Press.Google Scholar
  49. Katz, L. (1977). Reading ability and single-letter orthographic redundancy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(6), 653–659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1990). Kaufman brief intelligence test. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  51. Kemp, N., Parrilla, R., & Kirby, J. (2008). Phonological and orthographic spelling in high-functioning adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 15, 105–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Leslie, L., & Shannon, A. (1981). Recognition of orthographic structure during beginning reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 13(4), 313–324.Google Scholar
  55. Levy, B. A., Abello, B., & Lysynchuk, L. (1997). Transfer from word training to reading in context: Gains in fluency and comprehension. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 20, 173–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Levy, B. A., Bourassa, D. C., & Horn, C. (1999). Fast and slow namers: Benefits of segmentation and whole word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 115–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Lovett, M. W., Lacerenza, L., & Borden, S. L. (2000). Putting struggling readers on the PHAST track: A program to integrate phonological and strategy-based remedial reading instruction and maximize outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(5), 458–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Badha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(2), 129–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Markwardt, F. C. (1998). Peabody individual achievement test-revised/NU. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  61. Mason, J. (1980). When do children begin to read? An exploration of four year old children’s letter and word reading competencies. Reading Research Quarterly, 15(2), 203–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Massaro, D. W., Taylor, G. A., Venezky, R. L., Jastrzembski, J. E., & Lucas, P. A. (1980). Letter and word perception: Orthographic structure and visual processing in reading. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  63. Mather, N., Hammill, D. D., Allen, E. A., & Roberts, R. (2004). Test of Silent Word Reading Fluency (TOSWRF). Austin: Pro-Ed, Inc.Google Scholar
  64. Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatschneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40(2), 148–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Maurer, U., Brandeis, D., & McCandliss, B. D. (2005). Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1(1), 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Maurer, U., Brem, S., Krantz, F., Bucher, K., Benz, R., Halder, P., et al., (2006). Course neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read. NeuroImage, 33, 749–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 293–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. McCandliss, B. D., Posner, M. I., & Givo’n, T. (1997). Brain plasticity in learning visual words. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 88–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Meyer, M. S., & Felton, R. H. (1999). Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new directions. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 283–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Miles, T. R. (1983). Dyslexia: The pattern of difficulties. London: Granada.Google Scholar
  71. Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., Wolf, M., Sevcik, R. A., Steinbach, K. A., Frijters, J. C., et al. (2010). Multiple-component remediation for developmental reading disabilities: IQ, socioeconomic status, and race as factors in remedial outcome. Journal of Learning Disabilities (in press).Google Scholar
  72. Notenboom, A., & Reitsma, P. (2003). Investigating the dimensions of spelling ability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63(6), 1039–1059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Olson, R. K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B. J., & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In G. E. MacKinnon & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4). Orlando: Academic.Google Scholar
  74. Olson, R. K., & Wise, B. W. (1992). Reading on the computer with orthographic and speech feedback: An overview of the Colorado Remedial Reading Project. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 107–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Pammer, K., Lavis, R., Hansen, P., & Cornelissen, P. L. (2004). Symbol-string sensitivity and children’s reading. Brain and Language, 89, 601–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  78. Perfetti, C., Beck, I., Bell, L., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33(3), 283–319.Google Scholar
  79. Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  80. Plaut, D. C. (1997). Structure and function in the lexical system: Insight from distributed models of word reading and lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5/6), 765–805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Posner, M., & McCandliss, B. D. (2000). Brain circuitry during reading. In R. Klein & P. McMullen (Eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia (pp. 305–337). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  82. Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A., et al. (2008). Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(4), 353–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rashotte, C. A., & Torgesen, J. K. (1985). Repeated reading and reading fluency in learning disabled children. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(2), 180–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2(2), 31–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36(2), 321–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Roach, N. W., & Hogben, J. H. (2004). Attentional modulation of visual processing in adult dyslexia: A spatial-cuing deficit. Psychological Science, 15(10), 650–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Samuels, S. J. (1979). The method of repeated readings. The Reading Teacher, 32(4), 403–408.Google Scholar
  88. Scarborough, H. S. (1984). Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 329–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(2), 265–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Share, D. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Sharp, A. C., Sinatra, G. M., & Reynolds, R. E. (2008). The development of children’s orthographic knowledge: A microgenetic perspective. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(3), 206–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based tests of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 24–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Sunseth, K., & Bowers, P. G. (2002). Rapid naming and phonemic awareness: Contributions to reading, spelling, and orthographic knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 401–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.Google Scholar
  97. Therrien, W. J. (2004). Fluency and comprehension gains as a result of repeated reading. Remedial and Special Education, 25, 252–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Examiner’s manual. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  99. Treiman, R. (1997). Spelling in normal children and dyslexics. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 191–219). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  100. Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Tanzman, M. S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 279–329). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  101. Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the role of the magnocellular pathway in attention. NeuroReport, 10(6), 1283–1287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-third edition. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  104. Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (2001). Gray oral reading tests. Examiner’s manual. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  106. Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2004). Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests (RAN/RAS). Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  108. Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Wolf, M., Miller, L. T., & Donnelly, K. (2000). Retrieval, automaticity, vocabulary elaboration, orthography (RAVE-O): A comprehensive, fluency-based reading intervention program. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 375–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery tests-revised/NU. Examiner’s manual. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  111. Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
  112. Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Beth A. O’Brien
    • 1
  • Maryanne Wolf
    • 2
  • Lynne T. Miller
    • 2
  • Maureen W. Lovett
    • 3
  • Robin Morris
    • 4
  1. 1.School of Education, CECHUniversity of CincinnatiCincinnatiUSA
  2. 2.Tufts UniversityMedfordUSA
  3. 3.Hospital for Sick ChildrenTorontoCanada
  4. 4.Georgia State UniversityAtlantaUSA

Personalised recommendations