Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 61, Issue 1, pp 85–110 | Cite as

Dyslexic children show deficits in implicit sequence learning, but not in explicit sequence learning or contextual cueing

  • Gracia Jiménez-Fernández
  • Joaquín M. M. Vaquero
  • Luis Jiménez
  • Sylvia Defior
Article

Abstract

Dyslexia is a specific learning disability characterized by difficulties with accurate and/or fluent word recognition and by poor spelling abilities. The absence of other high level cognitive deficits in the dyslexic population has led some authors to propose that non-strategical processes like implicit learning could be impaired in this population. Most studies have addressed this issue by using sequence learning tasks, but so far the results have not been conclusive. We test this hypothesis by comparing the performance of dyslexic children and good readers in both implicit and explicit versions of the sequence learning task, as well as in another implicit learning task not involving sequential information. The results showed that dyslexic children failed to learn the sequence when they were not informed about its presence (implicit condition). In contrast, they learned without significant differences in relation to the good readers group when they were encouraged to discover the sequence and to use it in order to improve their performance (explicit condition). Moreover, we observed that this implicit learning deficit was not extended to other forms of non-sequential, implicit learning such as contextual cueing. In this case, both groups showed similar implicit learning about the information provided by the visual context. These results help to clarify previous contradictory data, and they are discussed in relation to how the implicit sequence learning deficit could contribute to the understanding of dyslexia.

Keywords

Automatization Dyslexia Intentional learning Reading disabilities Unconscious learning 

References

  1. Bennett, I. J., Romano, J. C., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2008). Two forms of implicit learning in young adults with dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(Learning, Skill Acquisition, Reading, and Dyslexia), 184–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ben-Yehudah, G., Sackett, E., Malchi-Ginzberg, L., & Ahissar, M. (2001). Impaired temporal contrast sensitivity in dyslexics is specific to retain-and-compare paradigms. Brain, 124(7), 1381–1395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Castles, A., & Colheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cohen, A., Ivry, R. I., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Conlon, E., Sanders, M., & Zapart, S. (2004). Temporal processing in poor adult readers. Neuropsychologia, 42(2), 142–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Defior, S., Fonseca, L., Gottheil, B., Aldrey, A., Rosa, G., Pujals, M., et al. (2006). LEE. Test de lectura y escritura en español. Buenos Aires: Paidós.Google Scholar
  11. Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 343–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eden, G. F., Stein, J. F., Wood, H. M., & Wood, F. B. (1995). Temporal and spatial processing in reading disabled and normal children. Cortex. Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 31(3), 451–468.Google Scholar
  13. Facoetti, A., & Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39(4), 352–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1999). Performance of dyslexic children on cerebellar and cognitive tests. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31(1), 68–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gombert, J. E. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning to read: Implication as for subtypes of dyslexia. Current Psychology letters: Behaviour, Brain and Cognition, 10(1). Retrieved 30 March 2006 from http://cpl.revues.org/document202.html.
  16. Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 525–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Howard, J. H., Jr., Howard, D. V., Japikse, K. C., & Eden, G. F. (2006). Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1131–1144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. International Dyslexia Association (IDA). Board of Directors (2002). What is dyslexia? Retrieved 12 November 2003 from http://www.interdys.org/FAQWhatIs.htm.
  19. Jiménez, L., Méndez, C., & Cleeremans, A. (1996). Comparing direct and indirect measures of sequences learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 948–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jiménez, L., & Vázquez, G. A. (2008). Implicit sequence learning in a search task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1650–1657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jiménez, L., & Vázquez, G. A. (2010). Implicit sequence learning and contextual cueing do not compete for central cognitive resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi:10.1037/a0020378
  22. Jiménez-González, J. E., & Hernández-Valle, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(1), 44–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kelly, S. W., Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for implicit sequence learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8(1), 43–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Benassi, M., Bolzani, R., Facoetti, A., Giovagnoli, S., et al. (2010). Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: A comparative study. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 863–872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Menghini, D., Hagberg, G. E., Caltagirone, C., Petrosini, L., & Vicari, S. (2006). Implicit learning deficits in dyslexic adults: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33(4), 1218–1226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 30, 159–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Developmental dyslexia: The role of cerebellum. Dyslexia, 5, 155–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2007). Procedural learning difficulties: Reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends in Neurosciences, 30(4), 135–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(9), 508–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 401–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pavlidou, E. V., Williams, J. M., & Kelly, L. M. (2009). Artificial grammar learning in primary school children with and without developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Perruchet, P., & Amorim, M. A. (1992). Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: Evidence against dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(4), 785–800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pothos, E. M., & Kirk, J. (2004). Investigating learning deficits associated with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 10(1), 61–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ram-Tsur, R., Faust, M., & Zivotofsky, A. (2006). Sequential processing deficits of reading disabled persons is independent of inter-stimulus interval. Vision Research, 46(22), 3949–3960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Raven, J. (1995). Matrices progresivas de Raven. Escala CPM. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
  38. Reber, A. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Reed, J., & Johnson, P. (1994). Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(3), 585–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rüsseler, J., Gerth, I., & Münte, T. (2006). Implicit learning is intact in adult developmental dyslexic readers: Evidence from the serial reaction time task and artificial grammar learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(5), 808–827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Schneider, W., Eschmann, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
  42. Serrano, F., & Defior, S. (2008). Dyslexia speed problems in a transparent orthography. Annals of Dyslexia, 58(1), 81–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. E. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Soto, P., Sebastián, N., & Maldonado, A. (1992). Retraso en lectura: evaluación y tratamiento educativo. Madrid: Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
  45. Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z., & Manis, F. R. (2004). Slower implicit categorical learning in adult poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(2), 281–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 88–104.Google Scholar
  47. Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Reliability of phonological and surface subtypes in developmental dyslexia: A review of five multiple cases studies. Current Psychology Letters, 10(1). Retrieved 30 March 2006 from http://cpl.revues.org/document248.html.
  48. Steffler, D. J. (2001). Implicit cognition and spelling development. Developmental Review, 21(2), 168–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Stoodley, C. J., Harrison, E. P., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 795–798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Stoodley, C. J., Ray, N. J., Jack, A., & Stein, J. F. (2008). Implicit learning in control, dyslexic, and garden-variety poor readers. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1 Learning, Skill Acquisition, Reading, and Dyslexia), 173–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Stoodley, C. J., & Stein, J. F. (2006). A processing speed deficit in dyslexic adults? Evidence from a peg-moving task. Neuroscience Letters, 399(3), 264–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Van der Leij, A., & Van Daal, H. P. (1999). Automatization aspect of dyslexia: Speed limitations in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demanding, and orthographic compensation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 417–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Vaquero, J. M. M., Jiménez, L., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). The problem of reversals in assessing implicit sequence learning with the serial reaction time tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 97–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vicari, S., Finzi, A., Menghini, D., Marotta, L., Baldi, S., & Petrosini, L. (2005). Do children with developmental dyslexia have an implicit learning deficit? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76(10), 1392–1397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Vicari, S., Marotta, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41(1), 108–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Waber, D. P., Marcus, D. J., Forbes, P. W., Bellinger, D. C., Weiler, M. D., Sorensen, L. G., et al. (2003). Motor sequence learning and reading ability: Is poor reading associated with sequencing deficits? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(4), 338–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wolf, P. H., Michel, G. H., Ovrut, G. F., & Drake, C. (1990). Rate and timing of motor coordination in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 349–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gracia Jiménez-Fernández
    • 1
  • Joaquín M. M. Vaquero
    • 2
  • Luis Jiménez
    • 3
  • Sylvia Defior
    • 1
  1. 1.Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la EducaciónUniversidad de GranadaGranadaSpain
  2. 2.Departamento de Psicología Experimental y Fisiología del Comportamiento, Facultad de PsicologíaUniversidad de GranadaGranadaSpain
  3. 3.Departamento de Psicología Social, Básica y MetodologíaUniversidad de Santiago de CompostelaSantiago de CompostelaSpain

Personalised recommendations