Annals of Dyslexia

, Volume 59, Issue 1, pp 55–77 | Cite as

Artificial grammar learning in primary school children with and without developmental dyslexia

  • Elpis V. Pavlidou
  • Joanne M. Williams
  • Louise M. Kelly
Article

Abstract

This paper explores implicit learning in typically developing and primary school children (9–12 years old) with developmental dyslexia using an artificial grammar learning (AGL) task. Two experiments were conducted, which differed in time of presentation and nature of the instructional set (experiment 1—implicit instructions vs experiment 2—explicit instructions). Repeated measures analysis of variance (group × grammaticality × chunk strength) showed a group effect only in experiment 1 (implicit instructions), with only the typically developing children showing evidence of AGL. There was a grammaticality effect (adherence to the rules) for both groups in the two experimental situations. We suggest that the typically developing children exhibited intact implicit learning as manifested in AGL performance, whereas children with developmental dyslexia failed to provide such evidence due to possible mediating cognitive developmental factors.

Keywords

Artificial grammar learning Children Developmental dyslexia Implicit learning 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thank you to all of the children who took part in this study and the parents and teachers for their consent and cooperation. Special thanks go to Dr. Eleni Ziori for her scientific input in designing the study. This study was undertaken when the first author was in receipt of a scholarship from I.K.Y. (The Greek State Foundation for Scholarships).

References

  1. Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Turken, U., & Waldron, E. M. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review, 105, 442–481. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 585–609.Google Scholar
  4. Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. British Psychological Society. (1999). Working party of the division of educational and child psychology of the british psychological society. Dyslexia, literacy and psychological assessment. Leicester: British Psychological Society.Google Scholar
  6. Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In E. Rosch, & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 170–211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Cleeremans, A. (1997). Principles for implicit learning. In D. Berry (Ed.), How implicit is implicit? Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Cleeremans, A. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 406–416. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01232-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Combert, J. E. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning to read: Implications as for subtypes of dyslexia. Current Psychology Letters (Special Issue on Language Disorders and Reading Acquisition), 10(1). http://cpl.revues.org/document202.html.
  10. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. (1994). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  11. Dienes, Z. (1992). Connectionist and memory array models of artificial grammar learning. Cognitive Science, 16, 41–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dienes, Z., & Berry, D. C. (1997). Implicit learning: below the subjective threshold. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 3–23.Google Scholar
  13. Don, A. J., Schellenberg, E. G., Reber, A. S., DiGirolamo, K. M., & Wang, P. P. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adults with Williams Syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1&2), 201–225. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN231&2_9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dulany, D. E., Carlson, R. A., & Dewey, G. I. (1984). A case of syntactical learning and judgment: How conscious and how abstract? Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 113, 541–555. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fawcett, A., & Nicolson, R. (2001). Dyslexia: the role of the cerebellum. In A.J. Fawcett (Ed.), Dyslexia: Theory and good practice. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
  16. Fletcher, J., Maybery, M. T., & Bennett, S. (2000). Implicit learning differences: A question of developmental level? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(1), 246–252. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gomez, R., & Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00003-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Graham, S. (2000). Should the natural learning approach replace spelling instruction? Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 235–247. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hayes, N. A., & Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Two modes of learning for interactive tasks. Cognition, 28, 246–276. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90015-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Howard Jr., J. H., & Ballas, J. A. (1980). Syntactic and semantic factors in the classification of nonspeech transient patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 28(5), 431–439.Google Scholar
  21. Howard Jr, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12, 634–656. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V., Japikse, J. C., & Eden, G. F. (2006). Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1131–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kelly, S. W., Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for implicit sequence learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 8, 43–52. doi: 10.1002/dys.208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kerchner, L., & Kistinger, B. (1984). Language processing/word processing: Written expression, computers and learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 7, 329–335. doi: 10.2307/1510232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1994). The information acquired during artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 79–91. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.20.1.79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1996). Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 169–181. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lewicki, P. (1986). Nonconscious social information processing. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
  28. MacArthur, C., Graham, S., Schwartz, S., & Schafer, W. (1995). Evaluation of a writing instruction model that integrated a process approach, strategy instruction, and word processing. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20, 189–210.Google Scholar
  29. Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants and neural networks. Science, 283, 77–80. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5398.77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mathews, R. C., Buss, R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchard- Fields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. (1989). Role of implicit and explicit processes in learning from examples: A synergistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1083–1100. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mathews, R. C., & Cochran, B. P. (1998). Project grammarama revisited: Generativity of implicitly acquired knowledge. In M. A. Stadler, & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 223–259). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  32. Maybery, M., Taylor, M., & O’Brien- Malone, A. (1995). Implicit learning: Sensitive to age but not IQ. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 8–17. doi: 10.1080/00049539508258763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Meulemans, T., & Van der Linden, M. (1997). Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1007–1028. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.23.4.1007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, P. (1998). Implicit sequence learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 199–221. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mostofsky, S. H., Goldberg, M. C., Landa, R. J., & Denckla, M. B. (2000). Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: Implications for cerebellar contribution. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 752–759. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700677020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nicolson, R., & Fawcett, A. (1999). Developmental dyslexia: The role of the cerebellum. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 5, 155–157.Google Scholar
  37. Nicolson, R., Fawcett, A. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory and dyslexia. Technical Report, LRG 3/91, Dept. of Psychology, University of Sheffield.Google Scholar
  38. Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirement of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1991). Implicit acquisition of abstract knowledge about artificial grammar: Some methodological and conceptual issues. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 120(1), 112–116. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.1.112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(3), 353–363. doi: 10.1037/h0025953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 227–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pothos, E. M., & Bailey, M. T. (2000). The role of similarity in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 26(4), 847–862.Google Scholar
  43. Pothos, E. M., & Kirk. (2004). Investigating learning deficits associated with dyslexia. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 10, 61–76. doi: 10.1002/dys.266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80149-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 88–94. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.2.1.88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 118(3), 219–235. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Reber, A. S. (1992). The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 93–133. doi: 10.1016/1053-8100(92)90051-B.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. (Oxford Psychology Series No.19). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Reber, A. S., Kassin, S., Lewis, S., & Cantor, G. (1980). On the relationship between implicit and explicit modes in the learning of a complex rules structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning and Memory, 6, 492–502. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.6.5.492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Reber, A. S., & Lewis, S. (1977). Implicit learning: An analysis of the form and structure of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition, 5(4), 333–361. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(77)90020-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(5), 888–896. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Roodenrys, S., & Dunn, N. (2007). Unimpaired implicit learning in children with developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 14(1), 1–15. doi: 10.1002/dys.340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Russeler, J., Gerth, I., & Munte, T. (2006). Implicit learning is intact in adult developmental dyslexics readers: Evidence from the serial reaction time task and artificial grammar learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(5), 808–827. doi: 10.1080/13803390591001007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sallas, B., Mathews, C., Lane, S. M., & Sun, R. (2007). Developing rich and quickly assessed knowledge of an artificial grammar. Memory & Cognition, 35(8), 2118–2133.Google Scholar
  55. Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit memory: A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 159–182. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  58. Sperling, J., Lu, Z. L., & Manis, F. R. (2004). Slower implicit categorical learning in adult poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(2), 281–303. doi: 10.1007/s11881-004-0014-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Stoodley, C. J., Harrison, E. P. D., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 795–798. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 249–283. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Thomas, K. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Serial reaction time learning in pre-school- and school-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 364–387. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Thomas, K. M., Welsh, T. F., Eccard, C. H., Livnat, R., Pierri, J. N., & Casey, B. J. (1998). Response inhibition in children with striatal lesions: A functional MRI study. Proceedings of the American Psychological Society 10th Annual Convention. Google Scholar
  63. Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J., & Hutton, U. (1998). A reevaluation of working memory capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(2), 195–217. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Vicari, S., Finzi, A., Menghini, L., Marrota, L., Baldi, S., & Petrosini, L. (2005). Do children with developmental dyslexia have an implicit learning deficit? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, 1392–1397. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.061093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Vicari, S., Marrota, L., Menghini, D., Molinari, M., & Petrosini, L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41, 108–114. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00082-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71(5), 1223–1240. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668–680. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The International Dyslexia Association 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elpis V. Pavlidou
    • 1
  • Joanne M. Williams
    • 2
  • Louise M. Kelly
    • 3
  1. 1.Moray House School of EducationUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  2. 2.Moray House School of EducationUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations