Advertisement

ZDM

, Volume 45, Issue 3, pp 425–440 | Cite as

Teachers’ gestures and speech in mathematics lessons: forging common ground by resolving trouble spots

  • Martha W. Alibali
  • Mitchell J. Nathan
  • R. Breckinridge Church
  • Matthew S. Wolfgram
  • Suyeon Kim
  • Eric J. Knuth
Original Article

Abstract

This research focused on how teachers establish and maintain shared understanding with students during classroom mathematics instruction. We studied the micro-level interventions that teachers implement spontaneously as a lesson unfolds, which we call micro-interventions. In particular, we focused on teachers’ micro-interventions around trouble spots, defined as points during the lesson when students display lack of understanding. We investigated how teachers use gestures along with speech in responding to such trouble spots in a corpus of six middle-school mathematics lessons. Trouble spots were a regular occurrence in the lessons (M = 10.2 per lesson). We hypothesized that, in the face of trouble spots, teachers might increase their use of gestures in an effort to re-establish shared understanding with students. Thus, we predicted that teachers would gesture more in turns immediately following trouble spots than in turns immediately preceding trouble spots. This hypothesis was supported with quantitative analyses of teachers’ gesture frequency and gesture rates, and with qualitative analyses of representative cases. Thus, teachers use gestures adaptively in micro-interventions in order to foster common ground when instructional communication breaks down.

Keywords

Gesture Classroom communication Common ground 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant # R305H060097 from the U. S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences (Alibali, PI). All opinions expressed herein are those of the authors and not the U. S. Department of Education. We thank Maia Ledesma, Kristen Bieda, and Elise Lockwood for their contributions to this research. Most of all, we thank the teachers and students who opened their classrooms to us and allowed us to videotape their instruction.

References

  1. Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2007). Teachers’ gestures as a means of scaffolding students’ understanding: Evidence from an early algebra lesson. In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B. Barron, & S. J. Derry (Eds.), Video research in the learning sciences (pp. 349–365). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from students’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Fujimori, Y. (2011). Gestures in the mathematics classroom: What’s the point? In N. Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental Cognitive Science Goes To School (pp. 219–234). New York: Routledge, Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
  5. Alibali, M. W., Young, A. G., Crooks, N. M., Yeo, A., Ledesma, I. M., Nathan, M. J., et al. (2012). Students learn more when their teacher has learned to gesture effectively (Submitted).Google Scholar
  6. Arzarello, F. (2006). Semiosis as a multimodal process. In Revista latinoamericana de investigacion en mathematic educativa, numero especial, pp. 267–299.Google Scholar
  7. Arzarello, F., Paola, D., Robutti, O., & Sabena, C. (2009). Gestures as semiotic resources in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 97–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Blake, B., & Pope, T. (2008). Developmental psychology: Incorporating Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories in classrooms. Journal of Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives in Education, 1(1), 59–67.Google Scholar
  10. Brintoon, B., & Fujiki, M. (1989). Conversational management with language-impaired children. Rockville: Aspen.Google Scholar
  11. Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., & Mahootian, S. (2004). The role of gesture in bilingual education: Does gesture enhance learning? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7, 303–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Church, R. B., Garber, P., & Rogalski, K. (2007). The role of gesture in memory and social communication. Gesture, 7, 137–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. A. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
  15. Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106, 1047–1058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cobb, P., Wood, T., & Yackel, E. (1993). Discourse, mathematical thinking and classroom practice. In E. Forman, N. Minick, & C. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 91–119). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Edwards, L. D. (2009). Gestures and conceptual integration in mathematical talk. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 127–141.Google Scholar
  18. Evans, M. A., Feenstra, E., Ryon, E., & McNeill, D. (2011). A multimodal approach to coding discourse: Collaboration, distributed cognition, and geometric reasoning. Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 6, 253–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Flevares, L. M., & Perry, M. (2001). How many do you see? The use of nonspoken representations in first-grade mathematics lessons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 330–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–596.Google Scholar
  21. Golab, J., Lind, C., & Okell, E. (2009). Conversation analysis of repair in interaction with adults who have acquired hearing impairment. In Hearing Care for Adults 2009. Chicago: Phonak.Google Scholar
  22. Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children's hands to adults' ears: Gesture’s role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology, 39, 509–520.Google Scholar
  23. Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., & Singer, M. (1999). What the teachers’ hands tell the students’ minds about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 720–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldin-Meadow, S., Wein, D., & Chang, C. (1992). Assessing knowledge through gesture: Using children’s hands to read their minds. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 201–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Goodwin, C. (2007). Environmentally coupled gestures. In S. Duncan, J. Cassell, & E. Levy (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimensions of language (pp. 195–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  26. Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1991). Sharing cognition through collective comprehension activity. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 331–348). Washington: APA.Google Scholar
  27. Holler, J., & Stevens, R. (2007). The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 26, 4–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the gesture as simulated action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 245–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hostetter, A. B., Bieda, K., Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., & Knuth, E. J. (2006). Don’t just tell them, show them! Teachers can intentionally alter their instructional gestures. In R. Sun (Ed.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1523–1528). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Hurtig, R. (1977). Toward a functional theory of discourse. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension. Norwood: Ablex.Google Scholar
  33. Kim, M., Roth, W.-M.,& Thom, J. (2011). Children’s gestures and the embodied knowledge of geometry. International Journal of Science & Mathematics Education, 9(1), 207–238.Google Scholar
  34. Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2001). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  35. Marrongelle, K. (2007). The function of graphs and gestures in algorithmatization. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26(3), 211–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McNeil, N. M., Alibali, M. W., & Evans, J. L. (2000). The role of gesture in children’s comprehension of spoken language: Now they need it, now they don’t. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 131–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  38. McNeill, D. (2010). Gesten der Macht und die Macht der Gesten (gestures of power and the power of gestures). In E. Fischer-Lichte & C. Wulf (Eds.), Gesten, Inszenierung, Aufführung und Praxis (Gesture, Staging, Performance, and Practice). Munich: Wilhelm Fink.Google Scholar
  39. McNeill, D., & Duncan, S. (2000). Growth points in thinking-for-speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141–161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nathan, M. J., & Alibali, M. W. (2011). How gesture use enables intersubjectivity in the classroom. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 257–266). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  41. Nathan, M. J., Eilam, B., & Kim, S. (2007). To disagree, we must also agree: How intersubjectivity structures and perpetuates discourse in a mathematics classroom. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 16(4), 525–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Nathan, M. J., & Kim, S. (2009). Regulation of teacher elicitations in the mathematics classroom. Cognition and Instruction, 27(2), 91–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. NCTM. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston: Author.Google Scholar
  44. Nemirovsky, R., Rasmussen, C., Sweeney, G., & Wawro, M. (2012). When the classroom floor becomes the complex plane: Addition and multiplication as ways of bodily navigation. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 287–323.Google Scholar
  45. Núñez, R. (2005). Do real numbers really move? Language, thought, and gesture: The embodied cognitive foundations of mathematics. In F. Iida, R. Pfeifer, L. Steels, & Y. Kuniyoshi (Eds.), Embodied artificial intelligence (pp. 54–73). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  46. Paulus, T. M. (2009). Online but off-topic: Negotiating common ground in small learning groups. Instructional Science, 37, 227–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Radford, L., Edwards, L., & Arzarello, F. (2009). Introduction: beyond words. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 91–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rasmussen, C., Stephan, M., & Allen, K. (2004). Classroom mathematical practices and gesturing. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 23, 301–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316, 1128–1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Roth, W.-M. (2002). Gestures: Their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71, 365–392.Google Scholar
  51. Schegloff, E., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organisation of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382.Google Scholar
  52. Schleppenbach, M., Flevares, L. M., Sims, L. M., & Perry, M. (2007). Teachers’ responses to student mistakes in Chinese and U.S. mathematics classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 108(2), 131–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1998). Toward a theory of teaching-in-context. Issues in Education: Contributions from Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1–94.Google Scholar
  54. Seedhouse, P. (2004). The interactional architecture of the language classroom: A conversation analysis perspective. Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  55. Sfard, A. (2009). What’s all the fuss about gestures? A commentary. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 191–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied cognition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  57. Sidnell, J. (2010). Conversation analysis: An introduction. West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
  58. Singer, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 16, 85–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Singer, M. A., Radinsky, J., & Goldman, S. R. (2008). The role of gesture in meaning construction. Discourse Processes, 45, 365–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. L. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 187–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Trans.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© FIZ Karlsruhe 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Martha W. Alibali
    • 1
  • Mitchell J. Nathan
    • 2
  • R. Breckinridge Church
    • 3
  • Matthew S. Wolfgram
    • 4
  • Suyeon Kim
    • 5
  • Eric J. Knuth
    • 6
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WisconsinMadisonUSA
  2. 2.Department of Educational PsychologyUniversity of WisconsinMadisonUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyNortheastern Illinois UniversityChicagoUSA
  4. 4.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of AlabamaTuscaloosaUSA
  5. 5.Anyang UniversityAnyangKorea
  6. 6.Department of Curriculum and InstructionUniversity of WisconsinMadisonUSA

Personalised recommendations