ZDM

, 43:269 | Cite as

Is this a coincidence? The role of examples in fostering a need for proof

Original Article

Abstract

It is widely known that students often treat examples that satisfy a certain universal statement as sufficient for showing that the statement is true without recognizing the conventional need for a general proof. Our study focuses on special cases in which examples satisfy certain universal statements, either true or false in a special type of mathematical task, which we term “Is this a coincidence?”. In each task of this type, a geometrical example was chosen carefully in a way that appears to illustrate a more general and potentially surprising phenomenon, which can be seen as a conjecture. In this paper, we articulate some design principles underlying the choice of examples for this type of task, and examine how such tasks may trigger a need for proof. Our findings point to two different kinds of ways of dealing with the task. One is characterized by a doubtful disposition regarding the generality of the observed phenomenon. The other kind of response was overconfidence in the conjecture even when it was false. In both cases, a need for “proof” was evoked; however, this need did not necessarily lead to a valid proof. We used this type of task with two different groups: capable high school students and experienced secondary mathematics teachers. The findings were similar in both groups.

References

  1. Alcock, L. (2004). Uses of example objects in proving. In H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.) Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the International Group for The Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 17–24). Bergen, Norway: Bergen University College.Google Scholar
  2. Balacheff, N. (1990). Towards a Problématique for Research in Mathematics Education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21(4), 258–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Behr, M., & Harel, G. (1995). Students’ errors, misconceptions, and conflict in application of procedures. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 12(3/4), 75–84.Google Scholar
  4. Buchbinder, O., & Zaslavsky, O. (2007). How to decide? Students’ ways of determining the validity of mathematical statements. In D. Pita-Fantasy & G. Philippot (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Congress of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education (pp. 561–571), Larnaca: University of Cyprus.Google Scholar
  5. Buchbinder, O., & Zaslavsky, O. (2008). Uncertainty: A driving force in creating a need for proving. Accepted to The International Commission on Mathematical Instruction (ICMI), Study 19: Proof and Proving in Mathematics Education.Google Scholar
  6. Buchbinder, O., & Zaslavsky, O. (2009). A framework for understanding the status of examples in establishing the validity of mathematical statements. In M. Tzekaki, M. Kaldrimidou, & C. Sakonidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 225–232). Thessaloniki, Greece.Google Scholar
  7. Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics: An educational approach. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel.Google Scholar
  8. Fischbein, E., & Kedem, I. (1982). Proof and certitude in the development of mathematical thinking. In A. Vermandel (Ed.), Proceedings of the 6th International Conference of the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 128–131). Antwerp, Belgium.Google Scholar
  9. Hadas, N., Hershkowitz, R., & Schwarz, B. B. (2000). The role of contradiction and uncertainty in promoting the need to prove in dynamic geometry environments. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 44(1 & 2), 127–150.Google Scholar
  10. Hadass, N., & Hershkowitz, R. (2002). Activity analyses at the service of task design. In Proceedings of the 26th International Conference of the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 49–56). Norwich, UK: University of East Anglia.Google Scholar
  11. Harel, G. (2007). The DNR system as a conceptual framework for curriculum development and instruction. In R. Lesh, J. Kaput, & E. Hamilton (Eds.), Foundations for the future in mathematics education (pp. 263–280). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  12. Harel, G., & Sowder, L. (2007). Toward comprehensive perspectives on the learning and teaching of proof. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and leaning. (pp. 805–842). Reston, VA: NCTM, Information Age Pub Inc.Google Scholar
  13. Healy, L., & Hoyles, C. (2000). A study of proof conceptions in algebra. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(4), 396–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Mariotti, M. A. (2006). Proof and proving in mathematics education. In A. Gutierrez & P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 173–204). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  15. Movshovitz-Hadar, N. (1993). The false coin problem, mathematical induction, and knowledge fragility. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 12, 253–268.Google Scholar
  16. Movshovitz-Hadar, N., Inbar, S., & Zaslavsky, O. (1986). Students’ distortions of theorems. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 8(1), 49–57.Google Scholar
  17. Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research & evaluation methods (3rd ed.). California: Sage Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
  18. Peled, I., & Zaslavsky, O. (1997). Counter-examples that (only) prove and counter-examples that (also) explain. Focus on Learning Tasks in Mathematics, 19(3), 49–61.Google Scholar
  19. Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  20. Tsamir, P., & Dreifus, T. (2005). How fragile is consolidated knowledge? Ben’s comparison of infinite sets. Journal of Mathematical Behaviour, 24, 15–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Zaslavsky, O. (2005). Seizing the opportunity to create uncertainty in learning mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 60, 297–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Zaslavsky, O. (2008). Meeting the challenges of mathematics teacher education through design and use of tasks that facilitate teacher learning. In B. Jaworski & T. Wood (Eds.), The mathematics teacher educator as a developing professional (Vol. 4, pp. 93–114). In T. Wood (Series Ed.), The international handbook of mathematics teacher education. The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  23. Zaslavsky, O., Nickerson, S. D., Stylianides, A., Kidron, I., & Winicki, G. (2011). The need for proof and proving: Mathematical and pedagogical perspectives. In G. Hanna & M. de Villiers (Eds.), Proof and proving in mathematics education. Vol. 15 of the New ICMI Study Series. Springer (in press).Google Scholar
  24. Zodik, I., & Zaslavsky, O. (2008). Characteristics of teachers’ choice of examples in and for the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 69, 165–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© FIZ Karlsruhe 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Technion-Israel Institute of TechnologyHaifaIsrael
  2. 2.New York UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations