Advertisement

Evolutionary Biology

, Volume 39, Issue 1, pp 12–24 | Cite as

The Transmission of Genes and Culture: A Questionable Analogy

  • Nicolas ClaidièreEmail author
  • Jean-Baptiste André
Focal Reviews

Abstract

Modes of cultural transmission are, by analogy with modes of genetic transmission, ways in which cultural information is transmitted between individuals. Despite its importance across the behavioral sciences and for theories of cultural evolution, no attempts have been made, to our knowledge, to critically analyze this analogy. We here aim at such detailed comparison and show that the fundamental role of modes of transmission in biology results mainly from two properties of genetic transmission: (1) what is transmitted generally does not influence the way in which it is transmitted; (2) there is a limited number of simple and stable modes. In culture however, modes of transmission generally lack these two fundamental properties. In particular, in culture it is often the rate of evolutionary change that determines the mode of transmission. We offer some tentative explanation regarding the origins of such a fundamental difference and we conclude that cultural transmission modes are not causal mechanisms that govern the transmission of culture but mere descriptions of the way culture happens to be transmitted at a given time in a given community. This shows the limit of the analogy between biological and cultural evolution and suggests that evolutionary models and theories differ substantially between the two domains.

Keywords

Cultural evolution Memetics Dual inheritance theory Social learning Imitation 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Peter Godfrey-Smith, Hugo Mercier, Denis Roze, Eoin O’ Sullivan and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

References

  1. Allison, P. D. (1992). Cultural relatedness under oblique and horizontal transmission rules. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 153–169.Google Scholar
  2. Atran, S. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural history: Towards an anthropology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Atran, S. (2001). The trouble with memes. Human Nature, 12, 351–381.Google Scholar
  4. Aunger, R. (2000a). Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Aunger, R. (2000b). The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos, 28, 445–481.Google Scholar
  6. Aunger, R. (2002). The electric meme: A new theory of how we think. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  7. Barton, C. M., & Clark, G. A. (1997). Evolutionary theory in archaeological explanation. Archeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association, 7, 3–15.Google Scholar
  8. Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., & Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 271, 1443–1450.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Binmore, K. G., & Samuelson, L. (1992). Evolutionary stability in repeated games played by finite automata. Journal of Economic Theory, 57, 278–305.Google Scholar
  10. Blackmore, S. J. (1999). The meme machine. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bloch, M. (2000). A well disposed social anthropologist’s problem with memes. In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Bloch, M. (2005). Essays on cultural transmission. New York, Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
  13. Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Nunn, C. L., & Towner, M. C. (2006). Cultural macroevolution and the transmission of traits. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 15, 52–64.Google Scholar
  14. Bowler, P. J. (2003). Evolution: The history of an idea. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  15. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  16. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  17. Bruner, E. M. (1956). Cultural transmission and cultural change. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 12, 191–199.Google Scholar
  18. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Feldman, M. W., Chen, K. H., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1982). Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science, 218, 19–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Chung, C. (2003). On the origin of the typological/population distinction in Ernst Mayr’s changing views of species, 1942–1959. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 34, 277–296.Google Scholar
  21. Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 89–111.Google Scholar
  22. Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T., & Mace, R. (2010). Rise and fall of political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467, 801–804.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Daly, M. (1982). Some caveats about cultural transmission models. Human Ecology, 10, 401–408.Google Scholar
  24. Danovitch, J. H., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Should you ask a fisherman or a biologist?: Developmental shifts in ways of clustering knowledge. Child Development, 75, 918–931.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: Murray.Google Scholar
  26. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Deecke, V. B., Ford, J. K. B., & Spong, P. (2000). Dialect change in resident killer whales: Implications for vocal learning and cultural transmission. Animal Behaviour, 60, 629–638.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  29. Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
  30. Distin, K. (2005). The selfish meme: A critical reassessment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Dumersan, T. M., & Ségur, N. (1866). Chansons nationales et populaires de France. Paris: Librairie de Garnier Frères.Google Scholar
  32. Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature advance online publication.Google Scholar
  33. Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11, 453–457.Google Scholar
  35. Feldman, M. W., & Zhivotovsky, L. A. (1992). Gene-culture coevolution: Toward a general theory of vertical transmission. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 89, 11935–11938.Google Scholar
  36. Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: The Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  37. Fracchia, J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1999). Does culture evolve? History and Theory, 38, 52–78.Google Scholar
  38. Gabora, L. (2011). Five clarifications about cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 61–83.Google Scholar
  39. Garland, Ellen. C., Goldizen, Anne. W., Rekdahl, Melinda. L., Constantine, R., Garrigue, C., Hauser, Nan. D., et al. (2011). Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. Current Biology, 21, 687–691.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Gerard, R., Kluckhohn, C., & Rapoport, A. (1956). Biological and cultural evolution some analogies and explorations. Behavioral Science, 1(1), 6–34.Google Scholar
  41. Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In S. Levinson & N. J. Enfield (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 229–255). Oxford: Berg Publishers.Google Scholar
  42. Gintis, H. (2006). The emergence of a price system from decentralized bilateral exchange. The BE Journal of Theoretical Economics, 6, 13.Google Scholar
  43. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2009). Darwinian populations and natural selection. USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Gogarten, J. P., & Townsend, J. P. (2005). Horizontal gene transfer, genome innovation and evolution. Nature Reviews. Microbiology, 3, 679–687.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J., & Greenhill, S. J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in pacific settlement. Science, 323, 479–483.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 3503–3514.Google Scholar
  48. Guglielmino, C. R., Viganotti, C., Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1995). Cultural variation in Africa: Role of mechanisms of transmission and adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 7585–7589.Google Scholar
  49. Haldane, J. B. S. (1932). The causes of evolution. Green, London: Longmans.Google Scholar
  50. Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: The Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69, 197–214.Google Scholar
  51. Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 277, 3715–3724.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88(4), 922–934.Google Scholar
  53. Hewlett, B. S., De Silvestri, A., & Guglielmino, C. R. (2002). Semes and genes in Africa. Current Anthropology, 43, 313–321.Google Scholar
  54. Hofbauer, J., & Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolutionary games and population dynamics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Hull, D. L. (2000). Taking memetic seriously: Memetics will be what we make it. In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2006). Evolution in four dimensions. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Jablonka, E., Lamb, M. J., & Avital, E. (1998). ‘Lamarckian’mechanisms in darwinian evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 206–210.Google Scholar
  58. Jain, R., Rivera, M. C., & Lake, J. A. (1999). Horizontal gene transfer among genomes: The complexity hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 3801–3806.Google Scholar
  59. Kawai, M. (1965). Newly acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese Monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates, 6, 1–30.Google Scholar
  60. Keil, F. C., Stein, C., Webb, L., Billings, V. D., & Rozenblit, L. (2008). Discerning the division of cognitive labor: An emerging understanding of how knowledge is clustered in other minds. Cognitive Science, 32, 259–300.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Kimura, M. (1956). Random genetic drift in a tri-allelic locus; exact solution with a continuous model. Biometrics, 12, 57–66.Google Scholar
  62. Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 10681–10686.Google Scholar
  63. Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 5241–5245.Google Scholar
  64. Kronfeldner, M. (2007). Is cultural evolution Lamarckian? Biology and Philosophy, 22, 493–512.Google Scholar
  65. Krutzen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., Bejder, L., & Sherwin, W. B. (2005). Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 8939–8943.Google Scholar
  66. Kuper, A. (2000). If memes are the answer, what is the question? In R. Aunger (Ed.), Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behaviour Brain Science, 23, 131–145.Google Scholar
  68. Lasker, G. W. (1985). Surnames and genetic structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Lewis, H. S. (2001a). Boas, darwin, science, and anthropology. Current Anthropology, 42, 381–406.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Lewis, H. S. (2001b). The passion of Franz Boas. American Anthropology, 103, 447–467.Google Scholar
  71. Lewontin, R. (1974). The genetic basis of evolutionary change. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  72. Lozada, M., Ladio, A., & Weigandt, M. (2006). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge in a rural community of northwestern Patagonia, Argentina. Economic Botany, 60, 374–385.Google Scholar
  73. Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. Child Development, 73, 1073–1084.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Lyman, R. L., & O’Brien, M. J. (2003). Cultural traits: Units of analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of Anthropological Research, 59, 225–250.Google Scholar
  75. Mace, R., & Holden, C. (2005). A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 20, 116–121.Google Scholar
  76. Malthus, T. R. (1809). An essay on the principle of population, as it affects the future improvement of society. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  77. Manrubia, S. C., & Zanette, D. H. (2002). At the boundary between biological and cultural evolution: The origin of surname distributions. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 216, 461–477.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Mascaro, O., & Sperber, D. (2009). The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112, 367–380.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the theory of games. New York, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  80. Mayr, E. (1959). Typological versus population thinking. Evolution and anthropology:A centennial appraisal. Washington: The Anthropological Society of Washington.Google Scholar
  81. Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
  82. McElreath, R., & Strimling, P. (2008). When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Current Anthropology, 49, 307–316.Google Scholar
  83. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2004). Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the Origin of Species. Evolution, 58, 1–11.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behaviour Brain Science, 29, 329–383.Google Scholar
  85. Metzgar, D., & Wills, C. (2000). Evidence for the adaptive evolution of mutation rates. Cell, 101, 581–584.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Migliano, A. B., Vinicius, L., & Lahr, M. M. (2007). Life history trade-offs explain the evolution of human pygmies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 20216–20219.Google Scholar
  87. Morin, O. (2010). La transmission culturelle: Questions philosophiques et methodes quantitatives dans l’etude des traditions. Paris: Ecole des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales.Google Scholar
  88. Nei, M. (1967). Modification of linkage intensity by natural selection. Genetics, 57, 625–641.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Nowak, M. A. (2006). Evolutionary dynamics: Exploring the equations of life. Cambridge: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
  90. Ohmagari, K., & Berkes, F. (1997). Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the western James Bay Cree women of Subarctic Canada. Human Ecology, 25, 197–222.Google Scholar
  91. Ohtsuki, H., Hauert, C., Lieberman, E., & Nowak, M. A. (2006). A simple rule for the evolution of cooperation on graphs and social networks. Nature, 441, 502–505.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Otto, S. P. (2009). The evolutionary enigma of sex. The American Naturalist, 174 Suppl 1, S1–S14.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Perry, S., Baker, M., Fedigan, L., Grosa Louis, J., Jack, K., MacKinnon, Katherine. C., et al. (2003). Social conventions in wild white faced Capuchin monkeys: Evidence for traditions in a neotropical primate. Current Anthropology, 44, 241–268.Google Scholar
  94. Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  95. Runciman, W. G. (2005). Culture does evolve. History and Theory, 44, 1–13.Google Scholar
  96. Schelling, T. C. (1978). Micromotives and Macrobehavior. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  97. Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Kirby, S. (2010). Language evolution in the laboratory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 411–417.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Shennan, S. J. (2002). Genes, memes and human history: Darwinian archaeology and cultural evolution. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
  99. Shennan, S. J., & Steele, J. (1999). Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioural ecological approach. In O. H. Box & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives (pp. 367–388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  100. Skyrms, B. (2004). The stag hunt and the evolution of social structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  101. Smith, K., & Kirby, S. (2008). Cultural evolution: Implications for understanding the human language faculty and its evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 3591–3603.Google Scholar
  102. Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man, 20, 73–89.Google Scholar
  103. Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  104. Sperber, D. (2002). Conceptual tools for a natural science of society and culture. Proceedings of the British Academy, 111, 297–317.Google Scholar
  105. Sperber, D., & Claidière, N. (2006). Why modeling cultural evolution is still such a challenge. Biological Theory, 1, 20–22.Google Scholar
  106. Sperber, D., & Claidière, N. (2008). Defining and explaining culture (comments on Richerson and Boyd, Not by genes alone). Biology and Philosophy, 23, 283–292.Google Scholar
  107. Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 40–46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  109. Takahasi, K. (1999). Theoretical aspects of the mode of transmission in cultural inheritance. Theoretical Population Biology, 55, 208–225.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Taylor, P. D., & Jonker, L. B. (1978). Evolutionary stable strategies and game dynamics. Mathematical Biosciences, 40, 145–156.Google Scholar
  111. Tehrani, J. J., & Collard, M. (2009). On the relationship between interindividual cultural transmission and population-level cultural diversity: A case study of weaving in Iranian tribal populations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 286.e282–300.e282.Google Scholar
  112. Victoria, R.-G., James, B., Laura, C.-M., Nuria, F.-P., Thomas, W. M., Sorush, P., et al. (2009). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: An empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 274–285.Google Scholar
  113. Wallace, A. R. (1905). My life: A record of events and opinions. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
  114. Weibull, J. W. (1997). Evolutionary game theory. Cambridge: The MIT press.Google Scholar
  115. Wheeler, M., Ziman, J., & Boden, M. A. (2002). The evolution of cultural entities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  116. Wright, S. (1968). Evolution and the genetics of populations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  117. Yasuda, N., Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Skolnick, M., & Moroni, A. (1974). The evolution of surnames: An analysis of their distribution and extinction. Theoretical Population Biology, 5, 123–142.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Zanette, D. H., & Manrubia, S. C. (2001). Vertical transmission of culture and the distribution of family names. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 295, 1–8.Google Scholar
  119. Zeh, J. A., & Zeh, D. W. (2005). Maternal inheritance, sexual conflict and the maladapted male. Trends in Genetics, 21, 281–286.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of PsychologyUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsScotland, UK
  2. 2.CNRS UMR 7525 Ecologie et Evolution, Ecole Normale SupérieureParisFrance
  3. 3.CNRS UMR 7206 Eco-anthropologie et ethnobiologie, MNHN, Univ Paris DiderotParisFrance

Personalised recommendations