Intergenerational sustainability dilemma and the degree of capitalism in societies: a field experiment

Special Feature: Original Article Sustainability Science and Implementing the Sustainable Development Goals
Part of the following topical collections:
  1. Special Feature: Sustainability Science and Implementing the Sustainable Development Goals

Abstract

Maintaining intergenerational sustainability is a minimum requirement for the existence of humankind, but it is now becoming one of the biggest challenges. Thus, it is necessary to understand what factors determine human preference and behavior for intergenerational sustainability. We hypothesize that ongoing modernization of competitive societies, which we call “capitalism,” affects individual social preferences and other factors of human nature, compromising intergenerational sustainability. To examine this hypothesis, we implement an intergenerational sustainability dilemma game with “imaginary future generation” (IFG) as a policy tool (to prime people for future generations) in two types of Bangladeshi fields: (1) urban (capitalistic) and (2) rural (less capitalistic) areas. The analysis reveals that the likelihood of choosing intergenerational sustainable options significantly increases with the number of prosocial people in one generation and a dummy variable of rural areas. Since a considerable portion of people in rural areas are prosocial, rural people are identified to choose intergenerational sustainable options much more frequently than urban people. Moreover, the IFG treatment is not effective for urban people, implying that some stronger devices shall be necessary in capitalistic societies. Overall, our findings demonstrate that as societies become more capitalistic, intergenerational sustainability shall be further compromised through the change in people’s social preferences and area-specific effects.

Keywords

Intergenerational sustainability Capitalism Social preference Culture and evolution 

Abbreviations

IFG

Imaginary future generation

ISDG

Intergenerational sustainability dilemma game

SVO

Social value orientation

References

  1. American Association for the Advancement of Science (2016) Rise of the city. Science 352:906–907CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bangladesh Bureau of Statistics (2011) Population and housing census. Technical report, Bangladesh Bureau of StatisticsGoogle Scholar
  3. Bangladesh Bureau of Statistics (2013) Economic census. Technical report, Bangladesh Bureau of StatisticsGoogle Scholar
  4. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  5. Brosig-Koch J, Helbach C, Ockenfels A, Weimann J (2011) Still different after all these years: solidarity behavior in East and West Germany. J Public Econ 95:1373–1376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheverud JM, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka Pygmies. Am Anthropol 88:922–934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Costanza R, Kubiszewski I, Giovannini E, Lovins H, McGlade J, Pickett KE, Ragnarsdottir KV, Roberts D, Vogli RD, Wilkinson R (2014) Time to leave GDP behind. Nature 505:283–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dawkins R (2006) The selfish gene. Oxford university press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  9. Dewan A, Corner R (2014) Dhaka megacity: geospatial perspectives on urbanisation, environment and health. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  10. Ehrlich PR, Kareiva PM, Daily GC (2012) Securing natural capital and expanding equity to rescale civilization. Nature 486:68–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fisher M, Irlenbusch B, Sadrieh A (2004) An intergenerational common pool resource experiment. J Environ Econ Manag 48:811–836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Madore KP, Schacter DL (2014) Future planning: default network activity couples with frontoparietal control network and reward-processing regions during process and outcome simulations. Soc Cogni Affect Neurosci 9:1942–1951CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gilbert DT, Wilson TD (2007) Prospection: experiencing the future. Science 317:1351–1354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Griggs D, Smith MS, Gaffney O, Rockstrom J, Ohman M, Shyamsundar P, Steffen W, Glaser G, Kanie N, Nobel I (2013) Sustainable development goals for people and planet. Nature 495:305–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Harbaugh WT, Krause K (2000) Children’s altruism in public good and dictator experiments. Econ Inq 38:95–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hauser OP, Rand DG, Peysakhovich A, Nowak MA (2014) Cooperating with the future. Nature 511:220–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Henrich J, Ensminger J, McElreath R, Barr A, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2010a) Markets, religion, community size and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327:1480–1484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010b) The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci 33:61–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Henrich J, McElreath R (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 12:123–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Henrich JR, Bowles S, Camerer CF, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R, Alvard M, Barr A, Ensminger J, Heinrich NS, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe FW, Patton JQ, Tracer D (2005) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci 28:795–855Google Scholar
  21. Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366:1168–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hikosaka O, Yamamoto S, Yasuda M, Kim HF (2013) Why skill matters. Trends Cognit Sci 17:434–441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hooper PL, Gurven M, Winking J, Kaplan HS (2015) Inclusive fitness and differential productivity across the life course determine intergenerational transfers in a small-scale human society. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 282:20142808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jones JH (2015) Resource transfers and human life-history evolution. Ann Rev Anthropol 44:513–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kamijo Y, Komiya A, Mifune N, Saijo T (2017) Negotiating with the future: incorporating imaginary future generations into negotiations. Sustain Sci 12:409–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kaplan HS, Robson AJ (2002) The emergence of humans: the coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:10221–10226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kim PS, McQueen JS, Coxworth JE, Hawkes K (2014) Grandmothering drives the evolution of longevity in a probabilistic model. J Theor Biol 353:84–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kinzig AP, Ehrlich PR, Alston LJ, Arrow K, Barrett S, Buchman TG, Daily GC, Levin B, Levin S, Oppenheimer M, Ostrom E, Saari D (2013) Social norms and global environmental challenges: the complex interaction of behaviors, values, and policy. BioScience 63:164–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kopps AM, Ackermann CY, Sherwin WB, Allen SJ, Bejder L, Kutzen M (2017) Cultural transmission of tool use combined with habitat specializations leads to fine-scale genetic structure in bottlenose dolphins. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 281:20133245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Labeyrie V, Rono B, Leclerc C (2014) How social organization shapes crop diversity: an ecological anthropology approach among Tharaka farmers of Mount Kenya. Agric Hum Values 31:97–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Leibbrandt A, Gneezy U, List JA (2013) Rise and fall of competitiveness in individualistic and collectivistic societies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:9305–9308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Milinski M, Semmann D, Krambeck H, Marotzke J (2006) Stabilizing the earth’s climate is not a losing game: supporting evidence from public goods experiments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:3994–3998CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Morgan TJH (2016) Testing the cognitive and cultural niche theories of human evolution. Curr Anthropol 57:370–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Moya C, Boyd R, Henrich J (2015) Reasoning about cultural and genetic transmission: developmental and cross-cultural evidence from Peru, Fiji, and the United States on how people make inferences about trait transmission. Topics Cognit Sci 7:595–610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. North DC (1990). Institutions, institutional change and economic performance. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  36. O’Brien MJ, Lyman RL, Mesoudi A, VanPool TL (2010) Cultural traits as units of analysis. Philos Trans R Soc B 365:3797–3806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ockenfels A, Weimann J (1999) Types and patterns: an experimental East-West-German comparison of cooperation and solidarity. J Public Econ 71:275–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ostrom E (1990) Governing the commons: the evolution of institutions for collective actions. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  39. Piketty T (2014) Capital in the twenty-first century. Belknap pressGoogle Scholar
  40. Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2008) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  41. Ross RM, Atkinson QD (2016) Folktale transmission in the arctic provides evidence for high bandwidth social learning among hunter-gatherer groups. Evol Hum Behav 37:47–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schniter E, Gurven M, Kaplan HS, Wilcox NT, Hooper PL (2015) Skill ontogeny among tsimane forager-horticulturalists. Am J Phys Anthropol 158:3–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593–1599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Shahrier S, Kotani K, Kakinaka M (2016) Social value orientation and capitalism in societies. PLoS One 11:e0165067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sherstyuk K, Tarui N, Ravago MLV, Saijo T (2016) Intergenerational games with dynamic externalities and climate change experiments. J Assoc Environ Resour Econ 3:247–281Google Scholar
  46. Soldati GT, Hanazaki N, Crivos M, Albuquerque UP (2015) Does environmental instability favor the production and horizontal transmission of knowledge regarding medicinal plants? A study in southeast Brazil. PLoS One 10:e0126389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sticht TG, Beeler MJ, McDonald BA (eds) (1992) The intergenerational transfer of cognitive skills, Vol. 1: Programs, policy, and research issues; Vol. 2: theory and research in cognitive science. Ablex Publisher, WestportGoogle Scholar
  48. Stulp G, Sear R, Barrett L (2016a) The reproductive ecology of industrial societies, part i. Hum Nat 27:422–444CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Stulp G, Sear R, Schaffnit SB, Mills MC, Barrett L (2016b) The reproductive ecology of industrial societies, part ii. Hum Nat 27:445–470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Szpunara KK, Spreng RN, Schactera DL (2014) A taxonomy of prospection: introducing an organizational framework for futureoriented cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:18414–18421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tam K (2015) Understanding intergenerational cultural transmission through the role of perceived norms. J Cross-cultural Psychol 46:1260–1266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Timilsina RR, Kotani K, Kamijo Y (2017) Sustainability of common pool resources. PLoS One 12:e0170981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28:675–691Google Scholar
  54. United Nations (2016) The sustainable development goals report 2016. Technical report, United NationsGoogle Scholar
  55. Van Lange PA, Bekkers R, Shuyt TN, Vugt MV (2007) From games to giving: social value orientation predicts donation to noble causes. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 29:375–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Van Lange PA, Bruin ED, Otten W, Joireman JA (1997) Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: theory and preliminary evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 73:733–746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Van Lange PA, Schippers M, Balliet D (2011) Who volunteer in psychology experiments? An empirical review of prosocial motivation in volunteering. Pers Individ Differ 51:279–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. WCED (1987) Report of the world commission on environment and development: our common future. Technical report, United NationsGoogle Scholar
  59. Wilson DS, O’Brien DT, Sesma A (2009) Human prosociality from an evolutionary perspective: variation and correlations at a city-wide scale. Evol Hum Behav 30:190–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Japan KK 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Research Center for Future DesignKochi University of TechnologyKochiJapan
  2. 2.School of Economics and ManagementKochi University of TechnologyKochiJapan
  3. 3.Urban InstituteKyushu UniversityFukuokaJapan
  4. 4.College of BusinessRikkyo UniversityTokyoJapan
  5. 5.Research Institute for Humanity and NatureKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations