Management International Review

, Volume 52, Issue 3, pp 341–363 | Cite as

Response Style Differences in Cross-National Research

Dispositional and Situational Determinants
  • Anne-Wil Harzing
  • Michelle Brown
  • Kathrin Köster
  • Shuming Zhao
Research Article

Abstract

  • International Management researchers often rely on surveys to collect their data. However, responses to survey questions can be biased by response styles, a respondent’s tendency to provide a systematic response to questions regardless of their content. Response styles vary across countries and individuals, but there is limited systematic research that investigateswhy they vary.

  • Our study investigates middle (MRS) versus extreme response styles (ERS), the tendency to use the middle or extreme categories on rating scales. We examine the impact of culture, different types of scale anchors and the level of knowledge of the topic in question on MRS and ERS.

  • We asked five groups of respondents (Chinese in China, Chinese in Australia, Anglo-Australians in Australia, and two groups of German students in Germany) to indicate on a 10-point scale whether certain employee attitudes or behaviour were more typically Australian (left-hand of the scale) or Chinese (right-hand of the scale). We then asked them how they would rate the performance (low to high on a 10-point scale) of an employee who displayed this attitude or behaviour.

  • Asian respondents showed higher MRS than Western respondents. When scale anchors referred to naturally opposing and mutually exclusive constructs (Australian versus Chinese) respondents showed more ERS than when they referred to level or degree of a construct (low-high performance). Knowledge of cross national differences resulted in higher ERS on behavioural questions but not on performance questions.

Keywords

Response styles Survey research Culture China Australia Germany 

References

  1. Allik, J., & McCrae, R. R. (2004). Toward a geography of personality traits: Patterns of profiles across 36 cultures.Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 35(1), 13–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baumgartner, H., & Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (2001). Response styles in marketing research: A cross-national investigation.Journal of Marketing Research, 38(2), 143–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bello, D., Leung, K., Radebaugh, R., Tung, R. L., & van Witteloostuijn, A. (2009). From the editors: Student samples in international business research.Journal of International Business Studies, 40(3), 361–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bond, M. H., & Yang, K.-S. (1982). Ethnic affirmation versus cross-cultural accommodation. The variable impact of questionnaire language on chinese bilinguals from hong kong.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13(2), 169–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brauer, M., Chambres, P., Niedenthal, P. M., & Chatard-Pannetier, A. (2004). The relationship between expertise and evaluative extremity: The moderating role of experts’ task characteristics.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buckley, P. J., Devinney, T. M., & Louviere, J. J. (2007). Do managers behave the way theory suggests? A choice-theoretic examination of foreign direct investment location decision-making.Journal of international business studies, 38(7), 1069–1094.Google Scholar
  7. Chami-Castaldi, E., Reynolds, N., & Wallace, J. (2008). Individualised rating-scale procedure: A means of reducing response style contamination in survey data?The Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6(1), 9–20.Google Scholar
  8. Chen, C., Lee, S.-Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among east asian and north american students.Psychological Science, 6(3), 170–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2000). Assessing extreme and acquiescence response sets in cross-cultural research using structural equations modelling.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(2), 187–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chun, K.-T., Campbell, J. B., & Yoo, J. H. (1974). Extreme response style in crosscultural research.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5(4), 465–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clarke, I (2000). Extreme response style in cross-cultural research: An empirical investigation.Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15(1), 137–152.Google Scholar
  12. Clarke, I., III (2001). Extreme response style in cross-cultural research.International Marketing Review, 18(3), 301–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Couch, A., & Keniston, K. (1960). Yea-sayers and nay-sayers: Agreeing response set as a personality variable.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60(2), 151–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cronbach, L. J. (1946). Response sets and test validity.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6, 475–494.Google Scholar
  15. Dolnicar, S., & Grün, B. (2007). How constrained a response: A comparison of binary, ordinal and metric answer formats.Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 14(2), 108–122.Google Scholar
  16. Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to account for cross-cultural response bias: A classification of score adjustment procedures and review of research in JCCP.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(3), 263–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gao, G., Kao, K., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1998).Communicating effectively with the Chinese. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  18. Gibbons, J. L., Zellner, J. A., & Rudek, D. J. (1999). Effects of language and meaningfulness on the use of extreme response style by Spanish-English Bilinguals.Cross-Cultural Research, 33(4), 369–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Greenleaf, E. A. (1992). Measuring extreme response style.Public Opinion Quarterly, 56(3), 328–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hall, E. T. (1976).Beyond culture. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  21. Hamamura, T., Heine, S. J., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008). Cultural differences in response styles:The role of dialectical thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(4), 932–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Harzing, A. W. K. & 32 country collaborators (2005). The use of english questionnaires in cross-national research: Does cultural accommodation obscure National Differences?International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 5(2), 213–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harzing, A. W. K. (2006). Response styles in cross-national survey research: A 26-country study.International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 6(2), 243–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harzing, A. W. K. & 26 Collaborators (2009). Rating versus ranking: What is the best way to reduce response and language bias in cross-national research?International Business Review, 18(4), 417–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hofstede, G. (1980).Cultures consequences. international differences in work-related values. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  26. Hofstede, G. (2001).Cultures consequences, comparing values, behaviors institutions and organizations across nations, (2nd Ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  27. House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (2004).culture, leadership, and organizations: The globe study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks: SageGoogle Scholar
  28. Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of culture and response format on extreme response style.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(3), 296–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Johnson, T. J., Kulesa, P., Cho, Y. I., & Shavitt, S. (2005). The relation between culture and response styles.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36(2), 264–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Johnson, T., O’Rourke, D., Chavez, N., Sudman, S., Warnecke, R., Lacey, L., & Horm, J. (1997). Social cognition and responses to survey questions among culturally diverse populations. In L. Lyberg, P. Biemer, M. Collins, E. de Leeuw, C. Dippo, N. Schwarz, & D. Trewin (Eds.),Survey Measurement and Process Quality (pp. 87–113). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  31. Lee, C., & Green, R. T. (1991). Cross-cultural examination of the fishbein behavioral intentions model.Journal of International Business Studies, 25(2), 289–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (1989). On the empirical investigation of dimensions for cross-cultural comparisons.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(2), 133–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. O’Donovan, D. (1965). Rating extremity: Pathology or meaningfulness?Psychol Rev, 72(1965), 802.Google Scholar
  34. Olivas-Luján, M. R., Harzing, A. W. K., & McCoy, S. (2004). September 11, 2001: Two quasi-experiments on the influence of threats on cultural values and cosmopolitanism. InternationalJournal of Cross-Cultural Management, 4(2), 211–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction.American Psychologist, 54(9),741–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (1984). Socially desirable response and acquiescence in a cross-cultural survey of mental health.Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 25(2), 189–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Shiomi, K., & Loo, R. (1999). Cross-cultural response styles on the Kirton adaptation–innovation inventory.Social Behavior and Personality, 27(4), 413–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Si, S. X., & Cullen, J. B. (1998). Response categories and potential cultural bias: Effects of an explicit middle point in cross-cultural surveys.International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 6(3), 218–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Smith, P. B. (2004). Acquiescent response bias as an aspect of cultural communication style.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(1), 50–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Steenkamp, J. B. E. M., & Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research.Journal of Consumer Research, 25(June), 78–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Stening, B. W., & Everett, J. E. (1984). Response styles in a cross-cultural managerial study.Journal of Social Psychology, 122(2), 151–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Takahashi, K., Ohara, N., Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (2002). Commonalities and differences in close relationships among the Americans and Japanese: A comparison by the individualism/collectivism concept.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(5), 453–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Trompenaars. (1997).Riding the waves of culture. Understanding cultural diversity in business. London: Nicholas Brealey Publishing.Google Scholar
  44. van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2003). Bias and Substantive Analyses. In J. A. Harkness, F. J. R. van de Vijver, & P. P. Mohler (Eds.),Crosscultural Survey Methods ( pp. 207–234). Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  45. van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (1997).Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
  46. van Herk, H., Poortinga, Y. H., & Verhallen, T. M. M. (2004). Response styles in rating scales: Evidence of method bias in data from 6 eu countries.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(3), 346–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Watkins, D., & Cheung, S. (1995). Culture, gender, and response bias: An analysis of responses to the self-description questionnaire.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(5), 490–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zax, M., & Takahashi, S. (1967). Cultural influences on response style: comparisons of Japanese and American college students.Journal of Social Psychology, 71(1), 3–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Gabler Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anne-Wil Harzing
    • 1
  • Michelle Brown
    • 1
  • Kathrin Köster
    • 2
  • Shuming Zhao
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Management & MarketingUniversity of MelbourneParkvilleAustralia
  2. 2.Department of Business Administration IIHeilbronn UniversityHeilbronnGermany
  3. 3.School of BusinessNanjing UniversityNanjingChina

Personalised recommendations