Advertisement

Morphology

pp 1–34 | Cite as

The plural interpretability of German linking elements

  • Roland Schäfer
  • Elizabeth Pankratz
Article

Abstract

In this paper, we take a closer theoretical and empirical look at the linking elements in German N1+N2 compounds which are identical to the plural marker of N1 (such as -er with umlaut, as in Häus-er-meer ‘sea of houses’). Various perspectives on the actual extent of plural interpretability of these pluralic linking elements are expressed in the literature. We aim to clarify this question by empirically examining to what extent there may be a relationship between plural form and meaning which informs in which sorts of compounds pluralic linking elements appear. Specifically, we investigate whether pluralic linking elements occur especially frequently in compounds where a plural meaning of the first constituent is induced either externally (through plural inflection of the entire compound) or internally (through a relation between the constituents such that N2 forces N1 to be conceptually plural, as in the example above). The results of a corpus study using the DECOW16A corpus and a split-100 experiment show that in the internal but not external plural meaning conditions, a pluralic linking element is preferred over a non-pluralic one, though there is considerable inter-speaker variability, and limitations imposed by other constraints on linking element distribution also play a role. However, we show the overall tendency that German language users do use pluralic linking elements as cues to the plural interpretation of N1+N2 compounds. Our interpretation does not reference a specific morphological framework. Instead, we view our data as strengthening the general approach of probabilistic morphology.

Keywords

Compounds Linking elements German Probabilistic morphology Usage data Split-100 task 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank (in alphabetical order) Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain, Matthias B. Döring, Jordan Lachler, and Ulrike Sayatz for valuable discussions and comments. Furthermore, we thank Ulrike for helping us recruit the participants for the experiment. Luise Rißmann did sterling work helping with the annotation of the concordances and supervising a majority of the experiments. We are, of course, fully responsible for all residual inadequacies, errors, and omissions.

Compliance with ethical standards

The split-100 rating experiment reported in this paper was completely anonymised. Participants signed standard consent forms and were given the option of asking questions about the nature of the experiment before and after they took part in it. They were granted the right to revoke their signed declaration of consent (through anonymised personal codes), which none of them did. Participants received no payment, but they were awarded credit in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a first-semester class in German linguistics at Freie Universität Berlin. The participants could specify their gender on a voluntary basis and they were given no pre-formulated options.

References

  1. Anderson, S. R. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arndt-Lappe, S., Bell, M. J., Schäfer, M., & Schlücker, B. (2016). Introduction: modelling compound properties. Morphology, 26, 105–108.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-016-9285-4. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Augst, G. (1975). Untersuchungen zum Morpheminventar der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Tübingen: TBL Verlag. Google Scholar
  4. Baayen, R. H. (2009). Corpus linguistics in morphology: morphological productivity. In A. Lüdeling & M. Kytö (Eds.), Corpus linguistics. An international handbook (pp. 900–919). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.  https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110213881.2.899. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Banga, A., Hanssen, E., Schreuder, R., & Neijt, A. (2012). How subtle differences in orthography influence conceptual interpretation. Written Language and Literacy, 15(3), 185–208.  https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.10.2.01han. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Banga, A., Hanssen, E., Neijt, A., & Schreuder, R. (2013a). Preference for linking element -en- in Dutch noun-noun compounds: native speakers and second language learners of Dutch. Morphology, 23(1), 33–56.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-013-9211-y. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Banga, A., Hanssen, E., Schreuder, R., & Neijt, A. (2013b). Two languages, two sets of interpretations: language-specific influences of morphological form on Dutch and English speakers’ interpretation of compounds. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(2), 195–220.  https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0007. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baroni, M., Bernardini, S., Ferraresi, A., & Zanchetta, E. (2009). The WaCky Wide Web: a collection of very large linguistically processed web-crawled corpora. Language Resources and Evaluation, 43(3), 209–226.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10579-009-9081-4. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bochner, H. (1984). Inflection within derivation. The Linguistic Review, 3, 411–421.  https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.1984.3.4.411. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bresnan, J. (2007). Is syntactic knowledge probabilistic? Experiments with the English dative alternation. In S. Featherston & W. Sternefeld (Eds.), Studies in generative grammar. Roots: linguistics in search of its evidential base (pp. 77–96). Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton.  https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110198621.75. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dressler, W. U. (1987). Morphological islands: constraints or preference? In R. Steele & T. Threadgold (Eds.), Language topics: essays in honour of Michael Halliday (Vol. 1, pp. 71–79). Amsterdam: Benjamins.  https://doi.org/10.1075/z.lt1.50dre. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dressler, W. U., Libben, G., Stark, J., Pons, C., & Jarema, G. (2001). The processing of interfixed German compounds. Yearbook of Morphology, 1999, 185–220.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3722-7_8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dudenredaktion (Ed.) (2006). Duden: Die deutsche Rechtschreibung (24th edn. Vol. 1). Mannheim: Bibliographisches Institut & F. A. Brockhaus. Google Scholar
  14. Fehringer, C. (2009). Wie wär’s mit einem richtigen Mädelsabend? Plural -s within compounds in colloquial northern German. Journal of Germanic Linguistics, 21(2), 149–165.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1470542709000233 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ford, M., & Bresnan, J. (2013). Using convergent evidence from psycholinguistics and usage. In M. Krug & J. Schlüter (Eds.), Research methods in language variation and change (pp. 295–312). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511792519.020. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fuhrhop, N. (1996). Fugenelemente. In E. Lang (Ed.), Deutsch-typologisch (pp. 525–549). Berlin: De Gruyter. Google Scholar
  17. Fuhrhop, N., & Kürschner, S. (2015). Linking elements in Germanic. In P. O. Mueller, I. Ohnheiser, S. Olsen, & F. Rainer (Eds.), Word-formation: an international handbook of the langauges of Europe, 1 (pp. 568–582). Berlin: De Gruyter.  https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110246254-034. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gaeta, L. & Schlücker, B. (Eds.) (2012). Das Deutsche als kompositionsfreudige Sprache: strukturelle Eigenschaften und systembezogene Aspekte. Berlin: De Gruyter. Google Scholar
  19. Gallmann, P. (1998). Fugenmorpheme als Nicht-Kasus-Suffixe. In Variation und Stabilität in der Wortstruktur: Untersuchungen zu Entwicklung, Erwerb und Varietäten des Deutschen und anderer Sprachen (pp. 177–190). Hildesheim: Olms. Google Scholar
  20. Haspelmath, M. (2010). Framework-free grammatical theory. In B. Heine & H. Narrog (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of grammatical analysis (pp. 341–465). Oxford: Oxford University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199544004.013.0014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hay, J. B., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 342–348.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.002. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hintze, J. L., & Nelson, R. D. (1998). Violin plots: a box plot-density trace synergism. The American Statistician, 52(2), 181–184.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2685478. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hope, A. C. A. (1968). A simplified Monte Carlo significance test procedure. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 30(3), 582–598. Google Scholar
  24. Kilgarriff, A., Baisa, V., Bušta, J., Jakubíček, M., Kovář, V., Michelfeit, J., Rychlý, P., & Suchomel, V. (2014). The Sketch Engine: ten years on. Lexicography, 1(1), 7–36.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40607-014-0009-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kirchner, R., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). A level playing-field: perceptibility and inflection in English compounds. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 54(1), 91–116.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0008413100001055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Koester, D., Gunter, T. C., Wagner, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Morphosyntax, prosody, and linking elements: the auditory processing of German nominal compounds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1647–1668.  https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042568541. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Köpcke, K. M. (1993). Schemata bei der Pluralbildung im Deutschen: Versuch einer kognitiven Morphologie. Tübingen: Narr. Google Scholar
  28. Köpcke, K. M. (1995). Die Klassifikation der schwachen Maskulina in der deutschen Gegenwartssprache – Ein Beispiel für die Leistungsfähigkeit der Prototypentheorie. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 14(2), 159–180.  https://doi.org/10.1515/zfsw.1995.14.2.159. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Krott, A., Schreuder, R., Baayen, R. H., & Dressler, W. U. (2007). Analogical effects on linking elements in German compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(1), 25–57.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960500343429. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kupietz, M., Belica, C., Keibel, H., & Witt, A. (2010). The German reference corpus DeReKo: a primordial sample for linguistic research. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, S. Piperidis, M. Rosner, & D. Tapias (Eds.), Proceedings of the seventh international conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC ’10) (pp. 1848–1854). Valletta, Malta: European Language Resources Association (ELRA). Google Scholar
  31. Kürschner, S. (2005). Verfugung-s-nutzung kontrastiv: zur Funktion der Fugenelemente im Deutschen und Dänischen. Tijdskrift voor Skandinavistiek, 26, 101–125. Google Scholar
  32. Libben, G., Jarema, G., Dressler, W. U., Stark, J., & Pons, C. (2002). Triangulating the effects of interfixation in the processing of German compounds. Folia Linguistica, 36(1–2), 23–43.  https://doi.org/10.1515/flin.2002.36.1-2.23 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mohanan, K. P. (1986). The theory of lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Reidel.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-3719-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Neef, M. (2015). The status of so-called linking elements in German: arguments in favour of a non-functional analysis. Word Structure, 8(1), 29–52.  https://doi.org/10.3366/word.2015.0072. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Neef, M., & Borgwaldt, S. R. (2012). In Gaeta and Schlücker (2012) (pp. 27–56).  https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110278439.27.
  36. Nübling, D., & Szczepaniak, R. (2013). Linking elements in German: origin, change, functionalization. Morphology, 23(1), 67–89.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-013-9213-9. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Peirce, J. W. (2007). Psychopy – Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Perkuhn, R., Keibel, H., & Kupietz, M. (2012). Korpuslinguistik. Fink, Paderborn. Google Scholar
  39. Piantados, S. T. (2014). Zipf’s word frequency law in natural language: a critical review and future directions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5).  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0585-6. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: the ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books. Google Scholar
  41. Pollard, C. (1996). The nature of constraint-based grammar, paper presented at the 10th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation (PACLING’96). Seoul, Korea: Kyung Hee University. Google Scholar
  42. R Core Team (2014). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Google Scholar
  43. Rigby, R. A., & Stasinopoulos, D. M. (2005). Generalized additive models for location, scale and shape (with discussion). Applied Statistics, 54, 507–554.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9876.2005.00510.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schäfer, R. (2016). Prototype-driven alternations: the case of German weak nouns. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory ahead of print.  https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2015-0051. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schäfer, R., & Bildhauer, F. (2012). Building large corpora from the web using a new efficient tool chain. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. U. Doğan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, & S. Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the eighth international conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC ’12) (pp. 486–493). Istanbul: ELRA. Google Scholar
  46. Schäfer, R., & Bildhauer, F. (in preparation). The COW16 web corpora. Google Scholar
  47. Scherer, C. (2012). Vom Reisezentrum zum Reise Zentrum – Variation in der Schreibung von N+N-Komposita. In Gaeta and Schlücker (2012) (pp. 57–81). https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110278439.57.
  48. Schlücker, B. (2012). Die deutsche Kompositionsfreudigkeit: Übersicht und Einführung. In Gaeta and Schlücker (2012) (pp. 1–25). https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110278439.1.
  49. Schmid, H., Fitschen, A., & Heid, U. (2004). SMOR: A German computational morphology covering derivation, composition, and inflection. In M. T. Lino, M. F. Xavier, F. Ferreira, R. Costa, & R. Silva (Eds.), Proceedings of the IVth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 4) (pp. 1263–1266). Lisbon: Universidade Nova de Lisboa. Google Scholar
  50. Schreuder, R., Neijt, A., van der Weide, F., & Baayen, R. H. (1998). Regular plurals in Dutch compounds: linking graphemes or morphemes? Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(5), 551–573.  https://doi.org/10.1080/016909698386438 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Šidák, Z. K. (1967). Rectangular confidence regions for the means of multivariate normal distributions. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 62, 626–633.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1967.1048293 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Siegel, D. (1979). Topics in English morphology. New York: Garland. Google Scholar
  53. Szczepaniak, R. (2016). Is the development of linking elements in German a case of exaptation? In M. Norde & F. V. de Welde (Eds.), Exaptation and language change (pp. 317–340). Amsterdam: Benjamins.  https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.336.11szc. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Verhoeven, E., & Temme, A. (2017). Word order acceptability and word order choice. In S. Featherston, R. Hörnig, R. Steinberg, B. Umbreit, & J. Wallis (Eds.), Proceedings of Linguistic Evidence 2016 – Empirical, Theoretical, and Computational Perspectives, Tübingen: Univeristät Tübingen.  https://doi.org/10.15496/publikation-19061. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wegener, H. (2003). Entwicklung und Funktion der Fugenelemente im Deutschen. Oder: Warum wir keine *Autosbahn haben. Linguistische Berichte, 196, 425–456. Google Scholar
  56. Wegener, H. (2005). Das Hühnerei vor der Hundehütte: von der Notwendigkeit historischen Wissens in der Grammatikographie des Deutschen. In E. Berner, M. Böhm, & A. Voeste (Eds.), Ein gross und narhafft haffen: Festschrift für Joachim Gessinger (pp. 176–187). Potsdam: Universitätsverlag Potsdam. Google Scholar
  57. Wellmann, H., Reindl, N., & Fahrmeier, A. (1974). Zur morphologischen Regelung der Substantivkomposition im heutigen Deutsch. Zeitschrift für deutsche Philologie, 93, 358–378. Google Scholar
  58. Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A. A., & Smith, G. M. (2009). Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Berlin: Springer.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-87458-6. CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Deutsche und niederländische PhilologieFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Deutsche Sprache und LinguistikHumboldt Universität zu BerlinBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations