Advertisement

Morphology

, Volume 16, Issue 2, pp 313–336 | Cite as

Effects of age on the acquisition of agreement inflection

  • Elma BlomEmail author
  • Daniela Polisšenská
  • Fred Weerman
Original Paper

Abstract

Grammaticality judgement tasks show that second language learners who started during childhood are significantly more accurate on judging inflection than learners who started after puberty [Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99; Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1991). Cognition, 39, 215–258; McDonald, J. (2000). Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 395–423. Production data confirmthat inflection is a bottleneck in adult language acquisition, and that they differ from child learners in this respect [Lardiere, D. (1998). Second Language Research, 14, 359–375; Prévost, P. (2003). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 65–97; Pre vost, P., & White, L. (2000). Second Language Research, 16(2), 103–133]. Although the observations suggest that the acquisition of inflection is influenced by age, there is no study that focuses on this particular issue nor is there an articulated explanation available for the observed age-related difference. In this contribution, we compare child L2 learners of Dutch to child L1 and adult L2 learners of Dutch in order to investigate effects of age on the acquisition of verbal and adjectival inflection. We hypothesize that adult agreement paradigms differ from child agreement paradigms, the reason being that adult learners cannot rely on syntactic cues, whereas children make reliable use of syntax in building paradigms. By effect, adult learners end up with non-targetlike small paradigms that contain underspecified suffixes. We focus on the types of errors in the three learner groups (child L1, child L2 and adult L2). Our empirical basis consists of results obtained in a series of production experiments.

Keywords

Critical Period Hypothesis acquisition agreement inflection 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adger, D. (2005). Combinatorial variation in inflection. Paper presented at the Workshop variation in inflection 2005, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  2. Avrutin, S. (2005). What can we learn from comparative psycholinguistics. Paper presented at Generative approaches to Language Acquisition, Utrecht, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  3. Beard, R. (1982). Is separation natural? Studia Gramatycne, VII, 119–133.Google Scholar
  4. Beard R. (1995). Lexeme-Morpheme base morphology. SUNY Albany Press, Albany, NYGoogle Scholar
  5. Besten, H. den (1989). From Khoekhoe Foreignertalk via Hottentot Dutch to Afrikaans: The creation of a novel grammar. In M. Pütz & R. Dirven (Eds.), Wheels within wheels; Papers of the Duisburg Symposium on Pidgin and Creole Languages (pp. 207–249). Frankfurt am Main.Google Scholar
  6. Blom, E. (2003). From root infinitive to finite sentence. Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University.Google Scholar
  7. Blom, E. (2007). Modality, infinitives and finite bare verbs in Dutch and English child language. Language Acquisition, 14(1), (in press).Google Scholar
  8. Blom, E. Testing the domain-by-age-model: Inflection and placement of Dutch verbs. In B. Haznedar & E. Gavruseva (Eds.), Current trends in child second language acquisition: A generative perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins (submitted).Google Scholar
  9. Blom, E., & Don, J. (2005). How real are rules of Impoverishment? Paper presented at the Variation in Inflection Workshop, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  10. Blom, E., & Polišenská D. (2006). Verbal inflection and verb placement in first and second language acquisition. Linguistik International 16, Variation in Sprachtheorie und Spracherwerb; Akten des 39, Maurice Vliegen (red.) (pp. 1–10). Amsterdam: Lang.Google Scholar
  11. Broekhuis, H. (1999). Adjectives and adjective phrases. Modern Grammar of Dutch Occasional Papers 2. T: burg.Google Scholar
  12. Brown R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Penguin, HarmondworthGoogle Scholar
  13. van Coetsem F. (1988). Loan phonology and the two transfer types in language contact. Floris, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  14. Deutch W., Wijnen F. (1985). The article’s noun and the noun’s article: Explorations into the representation and access of linguistic gender in Dutch. Linguistics 23(5): 793–810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Elman J. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition 48(1): 71–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Felser C., Roberts L., Marinis T., Gross R. (2003). The processing of ambiguous sentences by first and second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 453–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Franceschina F., Hawkins R. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In: Prévost P., Paradis J. (eds), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp.175-205Google Scholar
  18. Goad H., White L., Steele J. (2003). Missing inflection in L2 acquisition: Defective syntax or L1-constrained prosodic representation?. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 48: 243–263Google Scholar
  19. Goldowsky B.N., Newport E. (1993). Modeling the effects of processing limitations on the acquisition of morphology: The less is more hypothesis. In Mead J. (eds), The Proceedings of the 11th WCCFL. CSLI, Stanford, CAGoogle Scholar
  20. de Haan A. (1996). De verwerving van de morfologische finietheid in het Nederlands. Groningen, RUG: MA thesisGoogle Scholar
  21. Haeseryn W., Romijn K., Geerts G., de Rooij J., van den Toorn M.C. (1997). Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst. Martinus Nijhoff, GroningenGoogle Scholar
  22. Halle, M. (1997). Distributed morphology: Impoverishment and fission. In MITWPL 30: Papers at the Interface (pp. 425–449). MITWPL: Cambridge.Google Scholar
  23. Halle M., Marantz A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale K., Keyser S.J. (eds), The view from building 20. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, pp. 111–176Google Scholar
  24. Haberzettl, S. (2005). Der Erwerb der Verbstellungsregeln in der Zweitsprach Deutsch durch Kinder mit russischer und türkischer Muttersprache. Tübingen: Niemyer.Google Scholar
  25. Harley H., Noyer R. (1999). Distributed morphology. Glot International 4: 3–9Google Scholar
  26. Haznedar, B. (1997). L2 acquisition by a Turkish-speaking child: Evidence of L1 influence. In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual BUCLD (pp. 245–256). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA: Cascadilla PressGoogle Scholar
  27. Haznedar, B., & Schwartz, B. (1997). Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In E. Hughes, M. Hughes & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual BUCLD (pp. 257–268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
  28. Hollebrandse, B., & Roeper, T. (1996). The concept of Do-insertion and the theory of INFL in acquisition. In F. Wijnen & C. Koster (Eds.), Groningen assembly on language acquisition (GALA 1995). Groningen: Center for Language and Cognition Groningen.Google Scholar
  29. Johnson J., Newport E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology 21: 60–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Johnson J., Newport E. (1991). Critical period effects on universal properties of language: The status of subjacency in the acquisition of a second language. Cognition 39: 215–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jordens P. (1990). The acquisition of verb placement. Linguistics 28: 1407–1448Google Scholar
  32. van Kampen, J. (1997). First steps in WH movement. Doctoral Dissertation, Utrecht University.Google Scholar
  33. Kiparsky, P. (1973). Elsewhere in phonology. In S. Anderson & P. Kiparsky (Eds.), A Festschrift for Morris Halle. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  34. Kiparsky P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In: van der Hulst H., Smith N. (eds), The structure of phonological representations (I). Forie, Dondrecht, pp. 131–175Google Scholar
  35. Laloi, A., Spanjaard, R., & Styczynska, J. (2005). Verbal and adjectival flexion by child L2 learners of Dutch. Final paper for ‘Early versus late language acquisition’ University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  36. Lardiere D. (1998). Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent end-state grammar. Second Language Research 14: 359–375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lardiere, D. (2000). Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory (pp. 102–129) Blackwell.Google Scholar
  38. Marinis T., Roberts L., Felser C., Clahsen H. (2005). Gaps in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27: 53–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McDonald J. (2000). Grammaticality judgements in a second language: Influences of age of acquisition and native language. Applied Psycholinguistics 21: 395–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. McCarthy, C. (2007). Morphological variability in second language Spanish. Doctoral dissertation, McGill University.Google Scholar
  41. Myles F. (2004). From data to theory: The over-representation of linguistic knowledge in SLA. Transactions of the Philological Society 102: 139–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Monographs in applied psycholinguistics. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Pienemann M. (1998). Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. John Benjamins, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  44. Pienemann, M., Di Biase, B., Kawaguchi, S., & Håkansson, G. (2005). Processing constraints on L1 transfer. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycho-linguistic approaches. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Pinker S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MAGoogle Scholar
  46. Pinker S. (1986). Productivity and conservatism in language acquisition. In: Demopoulos W., Marras A. (eds), Language acquisition and concept learning. Ablex Publishing Corporation, Norwood, NJGoogle Scholar
  47. Poeppel D., Wexler K. (1993). The full competence hypothesis of clause structure in early German. Language 69: 1–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Prévost P. (2003). Truncation and missing surface inflection in initial L2 German. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25: 65–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Prévost P., White L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research 16(2): 103–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Polišenská, D. (2005). Dutch children’s acquisition of inflection. Paper presented at IASCL 2005, Berlin.Google Scholar
  51. Sabourin, L. (2003). Grammatical gender and second language processing. Doctoral dissertation, Groningen University.Google Scholar
  52. Schwartz B.D. (1992). Testing between UG-based and problem-solving models of L2A: Developmental sequence data. Language Acquisition 2: 1–19Google Scholar
  53. Schwartz, B. D. (2003). Why child L2 acquisition?. In van Kampen J. & S. Baauw (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003 (vol. 1, pp. 47–66), Utrecht.Google Scholar
  54. Schwartz B.D. (2004). Child L2 acquisition: Paving the way. Proceedings of the 27th annual BUCLD (pp. 26–50). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla PressGoogle Scholar
  55. Schwartz B.D., Sprouse R.A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the ‘full transfer/full access’ model. Second Language Research 12(1): 40–72Google Scholar
  56. Sorace A. (2005) Syntactic optionality at interfaces. In: Cornips L., Corrigan K. (eds). Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 46–111Google Scholar
  57. Snow C., Hoefnagel-Höhle M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second-language learning. Child Development 49: 1114–1128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Steinhauer K., Alter K., Friederici A. (1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience 2: 191–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Thomason S., Kaufman T. (1988). Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  60. Tsimpli, I. M. (2003). Features in language development. Paper presented at the Eurosla conference, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  61. Unsworth, S. (2005). Child L2, adult L2, child L1: Differences and similarities. Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University.Google Scholar
  62. van der Velde, M. (2005). Gender acquisition across languages: Dutch and French compared. Paper presented at IASCL 2005, Berlin.Google Scholar
  63. Verhoeven, L., Vermeer, A., & van de Guchte, C. (1986, 2002). Taaltoets allochtone kinderen: Diagnostische toets voor de mondelinge vaardigheid Nederlands bij allochtone kinderen van 5–9 jaar (TAK onderbouw). Tilburg: Zwijsen.Google Scholar
  64. Weerman F., Bischop J., Punt L. (2006). L1 and L2 acquisition of Dutch adjectival inflection. ACLC Working papers 1(1): 5–36Google Scholar
  65. Zuckerman, S. (2001). The acquisition of “optional” movement. Doctoral dissertation, Groningen University.Google Scholar
  66. Zink, I., & Lejaegere, M. (2002). N-CDI’s: Lijsten voor communicatieve ontwikkeling. Aanpassing en hernormering van de MacArthur CDI van Fenson et al. Leuven: Acco.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elma Blom
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Daniela Polisšenská
    • 1
  • Fred Weerman
    • 1
  1. 1.Amsterdam Center for Language and CommunicationUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Faculty of Humanities, Department of Dutch StudiesUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations