Abilities and affordances: factors influencing successful child–tablet communication

Research Article

Abstract

Using Luhmann’s communication theory and affordance theories, we develop a framework to examine how kindergarten-grade 2 students interact with tablet computers. We assessed whether cognitive ability and device configuration influence how successfully children use tablet computers. We found that children’s limited ability to direct their cognitive resources affects child–tablet communication (i.e., sending and receiving information to and from the device). While it may appear that children simply know how to use this technology, they are actually engaged in a systematic assessment of the device governed by their level of attentional maturity. Interestingly, tablet computers designed for adults result in a higher frequency of successful communication but prolonged communication was most likely to take place on child-focused tablet computers. It seems that communication success and user engagement are independent.

Keywords

Affordance Tablet computer learning Computer-mediated communication E-learning Educational technology Mathematics education 

Notes

Funding

Funding for this research was provided by the following Canadian Foundation for Innovation, Social Science and Humanities Research Council, and the University of Toronto’s Faculty of Information. A very special thanks to our research assistants Kaitlin Woodward and Emilia Bustos Alegria who demonstrated excellence in the data coding and provided helpful comments throughout the analysis phase. Finally, thank you to the Semaphore Research Cluster on Mobile and Pervasive Computing.

References

  1. Adolph, K. E., & Kretch, K. S. (2015). Gibson’s theory of perceptual learning. In H. Keller (Ed.), International encyclopedia of social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  2. Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews: Neursocience, 4, 829–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8, 485–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 75–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bijker, W. E., Hughes, T. P., & Pinch, T. J. (1987). The social construction of technological systems: New directions in the sociology and history of technology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Fisher, P. H., Dobbs-Oats, J., Doctoroff, G. L., & Arnold, D. H. (2012). Early math interest and the development of math skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 673–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegman, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  10. Gibson, E. J. (1970). The development of perception as an adaptive process. American Scientist, 58, 98–107.Google Scholar
  11. Gibson, E. J. (1989, July). Learning to perceive or perceiving to learn? Paper presented to the International Society for Ecological Psychology, Oxford.Google Scholar
  12. Gibson, E. J. (1992). How to think about perceptual learning: Twenty-five years later. In H. L. Pick, P. van den Broek, & D. C. Knill (Eds.), Cognition: Conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 215–237). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gibson, E. J. (2002). Perceiving the affordances: A portrait of two psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  14. Gibson, E. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). The development of perception. In J. H. Flavell & E. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive development (Vol. 3). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). The “visual cliff”. Scientific America, 202, 64–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
  17. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
  18. Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. Madison, WI: Hilldale.Google Scholar
  19. Hale, S., Rose, N. S., Myerson, J., Strube, M. J., Sommers, M., Tye-Murray, N., et al. (2011). The structure of working memory abilities across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 26, 92–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Holzinger, A., Searle, G., & Wernbacher, M. (2011). The effect of previous exposure to technology on acceptance and its importance in usability and accessibility engineering. Universal Access in the Information Society, 10, 245–260.Google Scholar
  21. Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the social-an introduction to actor-network-theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Lefevre, J. A., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Fast, L., Kamawar, D., & Bissanz, J. (2009). Home numeracy experience and children’s math performance in the early school years. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 41(2), 55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Luhmann, N. (1992). What is communication? Communication theory, 2(3), 251–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lunden, I. (2014, March). Gartner: 195M tablets sold in 2013, Android grabs top spot from iPad with 62 % share. Retrieved from TechCrunch http://techcrunch.com/2014/03/03/gartner-195m-tablets-sold-in-2013-android-grabs-top-spot-from-ipad-with-62-share/.
  25. Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The Differential assessment of children’s attention: The test of everyday attention for children (TEA:Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology, 42, 1065–1081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Matei, S. A., Faiola, A., Wheatley, D. J., & Altom, T. (2010). The role of physical affordances in multifunctional mobile device design. International Journal of Information Technology and Web Engineering (IJITWE), 5(4), 40–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McEwen, R., & Dubé, K. A. (2016). Intuitive or idiomatic: An interdisciplinary study of child–tablet computer interaction. Journal of the Association for the Information Science and Technology, 67, 1169–1181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Norman, D. A. (1999). Affordance, conventions, and design. Interactions, 6(3), 38–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Norman, D. A. (2002). The design of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  30. Orlikowski, W. (2007). Sociomaterial practices: Exploring technology at work. Organization Studies, 28, 1435–1448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pols, A. J. K. (2012). Characterising affordances: The descriptions-of-affordances-model. Design Studies, 33(2), 113–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Selwyn, N. (2011). Education and technology: Key issues and debates. New york: Continuum International Publishing Group.Google Scholar
  33. Shneiderman, B. (2003). Promoting universal usability with multi-layer interface design. Computers and the Physically Handicapped, 73, 1–8.Google Scholar
  34. Still, J. D., & Dark, V. J. (2010). Examining working memory load and congruency effects on affordances and conventions. Journal of Human Computer Studies, 68(9), 561–571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Still, J. D., & Dark, V. J. (2013). Cognitively describing and designing affordances. Design Studies, 34(3), 285–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Werheid, K., Hoppe, C., Thone, A., Muller, U., Mungersdorf, M., & Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The adaptive digit ordering test: clinical application, reliability, and validity of a verbal working memory test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 547–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wiseheart, R., Altmann, L. P., Park, H., & Lombardino, L. J. (2009). Sentence comprehension in young adults with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 151–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. You, H. C., & Chen, K. (2007). Applications of affordance and semantics in product design. Design Studies, 28(1), 23–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Educational Communications and Technology 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.McGill UniversityMontrealCanada
  2. 2.University of TorontoMississaugaCanada

Personalised recommendations