Advertisement

Educational Technology Research and Development

, Volume 58, Issue 5, pp 485–505 | Cite as

The effects of diagrams and time-compressed instruction on learning and learners’ perceptions of cognitive load

  • Raymond S. Pastore
Research

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of diagrams and time-compressed instruction on learning and learners’ perceptions of cognitive load. The following design factors, visuals (visuals and non-visuals) and time-compressed instruction (0%-normal paced, 25, and 50%) were presented to 216 university students to analyze learning in a multimedia environment. Participants listened to audio instruction of the heart and those in the visuals condition viewed 19 diagrams that corresponded to the verbal instruction. The dependent variables consisted of four achievement tests: drawing, identification, terminology, and comprehension. Review behaviors (back and replay buttons) and learners’ perceptions of cognitive load served as additional dependent variables. The results of this study indicate that listening to normal or moderately compressed (25%) instruction in a multimedia environment supports learning. At these speeds, cognitive load is not increased thus allowing learners to gain a conceptual understanding of the material.

Keywords

Time-compressed instruction Multimedia Multiple representations Cognitive load Podcasts Diagrams 

References

  1. Arons, B. (1992). Techniques, perception, and application of time-compressed instruction. In Proceedings presented at the conference, American voice i/o society (pp. 169–177). Retrieved April 1st, 2007, from http://coblitz.codeen.org:3125/citeseer.ist.psu.edu/cache/papers/cs/5993/ftp:zSzzSzftp.media.mit.eduzSzpubzSzbaronszSzTimeCompressionAVIOS92.pdf/arons92technique.pdf.
  2. Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The split attention principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia (pp. 135–146). NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Human memory: Theory and practice. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  4. Barron, A. E. (2004). Auditory instruction. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research for educational communications and technology (2nd ed., pp. 949–978). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  5. Benz, C. R. (1971). Effects of time-compressed instruction upon the comprehension of a visual oriented television lecture. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University.Google Scholar
  6. Bray, J. H., & Maxwell, S. E. (1982). Analyzing and interpreting significant MANOVAs. Review of Educational Research, 52, 340–367.Google Scholar
  7. Brunken, R., Plass, J. L., & Leutner, D. (2003). Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 53–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 293–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clark, J. M., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3(3), 149–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen, B. H., & Lea, R. B. (2004). Essentials of statistics for the social and behavioral sciences. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
  11. DeLeeuw, K. E., & Mayer, R. E. (2008). A comparison of three measures of cognitive load: Evidence for separable measures of intrinsic, extraneous, and germane load. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 223–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Duker, S. (1974). Time-compressed instruction. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow.Google Scholar
  13. Dwyer, F. M. (1965). An experimental evaluation of the relative effectiveness of selected visual illustrations in teaching science concepts to college freshmen. Unpublished Dissertation. The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA.Google Scholar
  14. Dwyer, F. M. (1972). A guide for improving visual instruction. Learning services, box 784. PA: State College.Google Scholar
  15. Dwyer, F. M. (1978). Strategies for improving visual learning. Learning services, box 784. PA: State College.Google Scholar
  16. Dwyer, F. M., & Lamberski, R. (1983). The instructional effect of coding (color and black and white) in facilitating students’ information acquisition and retrieval. Educational Communication & Technology Journal, 31, 9–21.Google Scholar
  17. Eilam, B., & Poyas, Y. (2008). Learning with multiple representations: Extending multimedia learning beyond the lab. Learning and Instruction, 18, 368–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Foulke, E. A., & Sticht, T. G. (1967). The intelligibility and comprehension of accelerated speech. In Proceedings of the Louisville conference on time compressed speech, United States (pp. 21–28).Google Scholar
  19. Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2004). Designing instructional examples to reduce intrinsic cognitive load: Molar versus modular presentation of solution procedures. Instructional Science, 32, 33–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. He, L., & Gupta, A. (2001). Exploring benefits of non-linear time compression. In Proceedings presented at the ACM international conference on multimedia, Ottawa, Canada (pp. 382–391).Google Scholar
  21. Heiman, G. W., Leo, R. J., & Leighbody, G. (1986). Word intelligibility decrements and the comprehension of time-compressed instruction. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(6), 407–411.Google Scholar
  22. Janse, E., Nooteboom, S., & Quene, H. (2001). Word-level intelligibility of time- compressed speech: Porsodic and segmental factors. The Netherlands, Utrecht Institute of Linguistics. Retrieved April 1st, 2007, from http://www.let.uu.nl/~Sieb.Nooteboom/personal/timecompressed.pdf.
  23. Jeung, H., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). The role of visual indicators in dual sensory mode instruction. Educational Psychology, 17(3), 329–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 351–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Koroghlanian, C. M., & Sullivan, H. J. (2000). Audio and text density in computer-based instruction. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 22(2), 217–230.Google Scholar
  26. Low, R., & Sweller, J. (2005). The modality principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia (pp. 147–158). NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Lucking, R. A., Purcell, S., & Christmann, E. R. (2006). Can you podcast? Science Scope, 30(1), 16.Google Scholar
  28. Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Mayer, R. E. (2005). Introduction to multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia (pp. 135–146). NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Mayer, R., & Anderson, R. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 444–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mayer, R., & Chandler, P. (2001). When learning is just a click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 390–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mayer, R. E., Dow, G. T., & Mayer, S. (2003). Multimedia learning in an interactive self- explaining environment: What works in the design of agent-based microworlds? Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 806–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more materials results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mayer, R. E., & Sims, V. K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 389–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Monroe, A. H., & Ehninger, D. (1974). Principles and types of speech compression. Glenview, IL: Scott Foreman.Google Scholar
  38. Moreno, R. (2006). When worked examples don’t work: Is cognitive load theory at an Impasse? Learning and Instruction, 16, 170–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. Journal of Education Psychology, 87(2), 319–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Ollerenshaw, A., Aidman, E., & Kidd, G. (1997). Is an illustration always worth ten thousand words? Effects of prior knowledge, learning style and multimedia illustrations on text comprehension. International Journal of Instructional Media, 24(3), 227–238.Google Scholar
  41. Olsen, J. S. (1985). A study of the relative effectiveness of verbal and visual augmentation of rate-modified speech in the presentation of technical material. Unpublished Dissertation. University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
  42. Orr, D. B. (1968). Time-compressed instruction––a perspective. Journal of Communication, 18(3), 288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Orr, D. B., & Friedman, H. L. (1967). The effect of listening aids on the comprehension of time-compressed instruction. Journal of Communication, 17(3), 223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Paivio, A. (1979). Imagery and verbal processes. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  46. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45(3), 255–287.Google Scholar
  48. Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1969). Concrete-image and verbal memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 279–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pallant, J. F. (2007). SPSS survival manual: A step-by-step guide to data analysis with SPSS. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  50. Pastore, R. (2008). Students’ perception of podcasts in the classroom. International Journal of Instructional Technology and Distance Learning, 5(12).Google Scholar
  51. Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 61–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Schnotz, W., & Kurschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 469–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schnotz, W., & Lowe, R. (2003). External and internal representations in multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction, 13, 117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sorden, S. D. (2005). A cognitive approach to instructional design for multimedia learning. Informing Science Journal, 8, 263–279.Google Scholar
  55. Stevens, J. (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  56. Stoten, S. (2007). Using podcasts for nursing education. The Journal of Continuing Education in Nursing, 38(2). Google Scholar
  57. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 351–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
  61. Taylor, S. E., Frankenpohl, H., & Pettee, J. L. (1960). Grade level norms for the components of the fundamental reading skills. EDL Research and Information Bulletin, 3. Google Scholar
  62. Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3(4), 257–287.Google Scholar
  63. Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 147–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Verhelst, W., & Roelands, M. (1993). An overlap-add technique based on waveform similarity (WSOLA) for high quality time-scale modification speech. Paper presented at the International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, Retrieved April 1st, 2007, from http://www.etro.vub.ac.be/Research/DSSP/Publications/int_conf/ICASSP-1993.pdf.

Copyright information

© Association for Educational Communications and Technology 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Slippery Rock UniversitySlippery RockUSA

Personalised recommendations