Advertisement

Psychometrika

, 73:321 | Cite as

From Zero to Sixty: Calibrating Real-Time Responses

  • Theodoro Koulis
  • James O. Ramsay
  • Daniel J. Levitin
Application Reviews and Case Studies

Abstract

Recent advances in data recording technology have given researchers new ways of collecting on-line and continuous data for analyzing input-output systems. For example, continuous response digital interfaces are increasingly used in psychophysics. The statistical problem related to these input-output systems reduces to linking time-varying covariates to a continuous response variate. Using real-time data obtained from an experiment in psychoacoustics, we showcase new statistical tools that incorporate dynamical elements of an input-output system. We employ functional data analysis (FDA) methods and a simple differential equation to analyze and model the continuous responses. Furthermore, we outline the issues involved in analyzing input-output systems when the exact form of the underlying mathematical model is not known. Finally, we develop a calibration method to facilitate inter-subject and intra-subject comparisons.

Keywords

input-output functional data analysis real-time data, differential equations response calibration 

References

  1. Boker, S.M., & Wenger, M.J. (2007). Data analytic techniques for dynamical systems. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  2. Chow, S.-M., Ferrer, E., & Nesselroade, J.R. (in press). An unscented Kalman filter approach to the estimation of nonlinear dynamical systems models. Multivariate Behavioral Research. Google Scholar
  3. Fitts, P.M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–91. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Grodins, F.S. (1963). Control theory and biological systems. New York: Columbia University Press. Google Scholar
  5. Jagacinski, R.J., & Flach, J.M. (2003a). Control theory for humans. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  6. Jagacinski, R.J., & Flach, J.M. (2003b). Control theory for humans: Quantitative approaches to modeling performance. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  7. Madsen, C.K., & Fredrickson, W.E. (1993). The experience of musical tension: A replication of Nielsen’s research using the continuous response digital interface. Journal of Music Therapy, 30, 46–3. Google Scholar
  8. Marken, R.S. (2001). Controlled variables Psychology as the center fielder views it. The American Journal of Psychology, 114, 259–81. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Newell, K.W., & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). Applications of nonlinear dynamics to developmental process modeling. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  10. Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–13. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Pinheiro, J.C., & Bates, D.M. (1995). Model building for nonlinear mixed-effects models. (Technical Report, No. 91). Madison: Department of Biostatistics, University of Wisconsin. Google Scholar
  12. Pinheiro, J.C., & Bates, D.M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York: Springer. Google Scholar
  13. Powers, W. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine Publishing. Google Scholar
  14. Powers, W. (1990). Control theory and statistical generalizations. American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 24–1. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ramsay, J.O., & Silverman, B.W. (2005). Functional data analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Springer. Google Scholar
  16. Ramsay, J.O., Hooker, G., Campbell, D., & Cao, J. (in press). Parameter estimation for differential equations: A generalized smoothing approach, with discussion. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. Google Scholar
  17. Runkel, P.L. (1990). Research method for control theory. American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 14–3. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Searle, L.V., & Taylor, F.V. (1948). Studies of tracking behaviour. I. Rate and time characteristics of simple corrective movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 615–31. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Stevens, S.S., & Volkmann, J. (1940). The relation of pitch to frequency: A revised scale. American Journal of Psychology, 53, 329–53. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Taylor, F.V., & Birmingham, H.P. (1948). Studies of tracking behavior. II. The acceleration pattern of quick manual corrective responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 783–95. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Tustin, A. (1952). Feedback. In Mathematical thinking in behavioral sciences: Readings from Scientific American (pp. 66–3). San Francisco: Freeman (with introductions by D.M. Messick (1968)). Google Scholar
  22. Vines, B.W., Nuzzo, R.L., & Levitin, D.J. (2005). Analyzing temporal dynamics in music: Differential calculus, physics, and functional data analysis techniques. Music Perception. Google Scholar
  23. Vines, B.W., Krumhansl, C.L., Wanderley, M.M., & Levitin, D.J. (2006). Cross-modal interactions in the perception of musical performance. Cognition, 101, 80–13. CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Walls, A.T., & Schafer, J.L. (2006). Models for intensive longitudinal data. New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  25. Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. New York: Wiley. Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychometric Society 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Theodoro Koulis
    • 1
  • James O. Ramsay
    • 1
  • Daniel J. Levitin
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMcGill UniversityMontrealCanada

Personalised recommendations